posts: 29766
Data license: CC-BY
This data as json
id | title | slug | type | status | content | archieml | archieml_update_statistics | published_at | updated_at | gdocSuccessorId | authors | excerpt | created_at_in_wordpress | updated_at_in_wordpress | featured_image | formattingOptions | markdown | wpApiSnapshot |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
29766 | Loneliness and Social Connections | social-connections-and-loneliness | page | publish | <!-- wp:html --> <div class="blog-info">This article was first published in February 2020.</div> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Available research shows that social connections are important for our well-being. Having support from family and friends is important for our happiness and health, and is also instrumental to our ability to share information, learn from others, and seize economic opportunities.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections across countries and over time, and review available evidence on how and why social connections and loneliness affect our health and emotional welfare, as well as our material well-being.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Despite the fact that there is a clear link between social connections and well-being, more research is needed to understand causal mechanisms, effect sizes and changes over time. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>As we show here, oversimplified narratives that compare loneliness with smoking, or that claim we are living a 'loneliness epidemic', are wrong and unhelpful. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:owid/summary --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><a href="http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#how-important-are-social-connections-for-our-health">Social connections matter directly for our happiness and health.</a></li><li><a href="http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#the-importance-of-personal-relations-for-economic-outcomes">Social connections are instrumental to prosperity and material well-being.</a></li><li><a href="http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#causality-and-implications">Despite the clear link between social connections and well-being, more research is needed to understand causal mechanisms and effect sizes. In particular, more research is needed to pin down the effect that loneliness has on life expectancy.</a></li><li><a href="http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#loneliness-solitude-and-social-isolation">Solitary living arrangements are becoming increasingly common around the world, but there is no evidence that self-reported loneliness is increasing over time. This can be explained by the fact that loneliness and aloneness are not the same.</a></li><li><a href="http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#the-rise-of-social-media-platforms">Social media platforms have transformed social connections around the world. </a></li><li><a href="http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#are-facebook-and-other-social-media-platforms-bad-for-our-well-being">The causal effect that social media has on adolescent well-being is likely negative, but it is also likely heterogeneous across the population, and much smaller than newspaper headlines suggest.</a></li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- /wp:owid/summary --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><strong>Related articles</strong></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/trust">Interpersonal Trust</a> – Trust is an essential part of social connections. Trust is crucial for community, wellbeing, and effective cooperation.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/internet">Internet</a> – For many, the internet is now essential for work, finding information, and connecting with others.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta">Global Health</a> – Social connections matter for health. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in global health.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction">Happiness and Life Satisfaction</a> – Social connections matter for our happiness. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in happiness and life satisfaction across societies and locations.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading --> <h2>Social connections</h2> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>How important are social connections for our health?</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 29768 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Dr. Vivek Murthy, former <a href="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Surgeon_General_of_the_United_States" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">Surgeon General</a> of the United States, recently <a href="https://www.vivekmurthy.com/single-post/2017/10/10/Work-and-the-Loneliness-Epidemic-Harvard-Business-Review" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">wrote</a>: “Loneliness and weak social connections are associated with a reduction in lifespan similar to that caused by smoking 15 cigarettes a day”.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This ‘15 cigarettes a day’ figure has been reproduced and reported in the news many times, under headlines such as “Loneliness is as lethal as smoking 15 cigarettes per day”.{ref}There are dozens of articles reporting the ‘15 cigarettes a day’ figure. See for example <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://www.webmd.com/balance/news/20180504/loneliness-rivals-obesity-smoking-as-health-risk" target="_blank">this in WebMD</a>, and <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-5181559/Loneliness-bad-health-smoking-15-day.html" target="_blank">this in The Daily Mail</a>. From what I gather, all the articles that cite this figure trace back to Dr. Vivek Murthy and a meta-analysis by Holt-Lunstad et al. (2015), which I discuss in more detail below in this post. When I reviewed this meta-analysis I did find the claim that loneliness is a risk factor comparable to smoking; but I could not find the calculations that lead to the 15 cigarettes benchmark. It’s still unclear to me what the unit of loneliness is and what this translates into in terms of years of lives lost. However, given that 15 cigarettes per day is <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/consumption-per-smoker-per-day-bounds" target="_blank">about average consumption among smokers</a>, it’s possible that the benchmark comes from comparing excess mortality of smoking for the average smoker, vs. excess mortality of loneliness for average levels of loneliness.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>It is indeed quite a shocking comparison since around <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/smoking-and-secondhand-deaths" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">7 million deaths</a> globally are attributed to smoking every year, and back-of-the-envelope calculations published in medical journals say <a href="https://www.bmj.com/content/bmj/320/7226/53.1.full.pdf?casa_token=Fcl9ywjt31gAAAAA:9FnQFmC50vZclWEndIVf8Vf4uwlEsKIiQeeQtFXm11_06jJwLcgZn1MS4LPyFsFVnrcCsSSHXnv5" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">one cigarette reduces your lifespan by 11 minutes</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Here we dig deeper to try to understand what the data and research tell us about the link between social relations and health. In a nutshell, my reading of the evidence is as follows:</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:list {"ordered":true} --> <ol><li>There is a huge amount of evidence showing individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in their life.</li><li>There is credible theory and explanation of biological mechanisms, whereby isolation can set off unconscious surveillance for social threat, producing cognitive biases, reducing sleep and affecting hormones.</li><li>It's very likely there is a causal link, but there is no credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect that loneliness has on key metrics of health, such as life expectancy.</li><li>The fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect of loneliness on health doesn't mean we should dismiss the available evidence. But it does show that more research is needed.</li></ol> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Observational studies: A first look at the data</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":5} --> <h5>Measuring loneliness</h5> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Psychologists and social neuroscientists often refer to loneliness as <a href="https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2008-07755-000" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer"><em>painful</em> isolation</a>. The emphasis on painful is there to make a clear distinction between solitude – the state of being alone – and subjective loneliness, which is the distressing feeling that comes from unmet expectations of the types of interpersonal relationships we wish to have.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Researchers use several kinds of data to measure solitude and loneliness. The most common source of data are surveys where people are asked about different aspects of their lives, including whether they live alone, how much time they spend with other people in a given window of time (e.g. ‘last week’) or specific context (e.g. ‘at social events, clubs or places of worship’); and whether they experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. ‘I have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with’). Researchers sometimes study these survey responses separately, but often they also aggregate them in a composite index.{ref}The most popular composite index is the <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/UCLA_Loneliness_Scale" target="_blank">UCLA Loneliness Scale</a>, which was first proposed in 1978, and is based on a 20-item scale designed to measure subjective feelings of loneliness. You can read more about it <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://fetzer.org/sites/default/files/images/stories/pdf/selfmeasures/Self_Measures_for_Loneliness_and_Interpersonal_Problems_UCLA_LONELINESS.pdf" target="_blank">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Surveys <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0898264312460275" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">confirm</a> that people respond differently to questions about subjective loneliness and physical social isolation, which suggests people do understand these as two distinct issues.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the chart here I've put together estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness from various sources. The fact that we see such high levels of loneliness, with substantial divergence across countries, explains why this is an important and active research area. Indeed, there are literally hundreds of papers that have used survey data to explore the link between loneliness, solitude, and health. Below is an overview of what these studies find.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <figure><iframe style="width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;" src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults"></iframe></figure> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":5} --> <h5>The link between loneliness and physical health</h5> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Most papers studying the link between loneliness and health find that both objective solitude (e.g. living alone) and subjective loneliness (e.g. frequent self-reported feelings of loneliness) are correlated with higher morbidity (i.e. illness) and higher mortality (i.e. likelihood of death).</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The relationship between health and loneliness can of course go both ways: lonely people may see their health deteriorate with time; but it may also be the case that people who suffer from poor health end up feeling more lonely later down the line.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Because of this two-way relationship it’s important to go beyond cross-sectional correlations and focus on longitudinal studies – these are studies where researchers track the same individuals over time to see if loneliness predicts illness or mortality in the future, after controlling for baseline behaviors and health status.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The evidence from longitudinal studies shows that people who experience loneliness during a period of their life tend to be more likely to have worse health later down the line. In the Netherlands, for example, <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="https://academic.oup.com/aje/article-pdf/146/6/510/455063/146-6-510.pdf" target="_blank">researchers found</a> that self-reported loneliness among adults aged 55-85 predicted mortality several months later, and this was true after controlling for age, sex, chronic diseases, alcohol use, smoking, self-assessed health condition, and functional limitations.{ref}Penninx, B. W., Van Tilburg, T., Kriegsman, D. M., Deeg, D. J., Boeke, A. J. P., & van Eijk, J. T. M. (1997). Effects of social support and personal coping resources on mortality in older age: The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. American journal of epidemiology, 146(6), 510-519.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Most studies focus either on subjective loneliness, or on objective isolation. But some studies try to compare both. In a recent meta-analysis covering 70 longitudinal studies, the authors <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691614568352" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">write</a>:<em> “We found no differences between measures of objective and subjective social isolation. Results remain consistent across gender, length of follow-up, and world region.”</em> And in the concluding section they highlight that, in their interpretation of the evidence, <em>“the risk associated with social isolation and loneliness is comparable with well-established risk factors for mortality”</em>; which include smoking and obesity.{ref}Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on psychological science, 10(2), 227-237.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":5} --> <h5>The link between mental health and subjective well-being</h5> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In another much-cited review of the evidence, Louise Hawkley and John Cacioppo, two leading experts on this topic, <a href="https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3874845/" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">concluded</a> that <em>“perhaps the most striking finding in this literature is the breadth of emotional and cognitive processes and outcomes that seem susceptible to the influence of loneliness”.</em>{ref}Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of behavioral medicine, 40(2), 218-227. {/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Researchers have found that loneliness correlates with subsequent increases in symptoms related to <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/dementia-death-rates" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">dementia</a>, <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health#depression" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">depression</a>, and many other issues related to <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">mental health</a>; and this holds after controlling for demographic variables, objective social isolation, stress, and baseline levels of cognitive function.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>There is also research that suggests a link between loneliness and lower <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction" target="_blank">happiness</a>, and we discuss this in more detail <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-friends">here</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Mechanisms</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Experiments with social animals, like rats, <a href="https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/106/52/22393.full.pdf" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">show</a> that induced isolation can lead to a higher risk of death from cancer. Humans and rats are of course very different; but experts, such as Hawkley and Cacioppo, argue that these experiments are important because they tell us something meaningful about a shared biological mechanism. In a review of the evidence, Susan Pinker writes: “If our big brains evolved to interact, loneliness would be an early warning system—a built-in alarm that sent a biological signal to members who had somehow become separated from the group”.{ref}Pinker, S. (2015). The village effect: How face-to-face contact can make us healthier and happier. Vintage Canada.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Indeed, there’s evidence of social regulation of gene expression in humans: <a href="https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/108/7/3080.full.pdf" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">studies suggest</a> perceived loneliness can switch on/off genes that regulate our immune systems, and it is this what then affects the health of humans, or other animals that evolved with similar defence mechanisms.{ref}Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). Transcript origin analysis identifies antigen-presenting cells as primary targets of socially regulated gene expression in leukocytes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 3080-3085.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Causality and implications</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The bulk of evidence from observational studies and biological mechanisms, described above, implies that loneliness most likely matters for our health and well being. But do we really know how much it matters relative to other important risk factors?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The key point here is that estimates are likely biased to some extent.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The findings from longitudinal studies that track individuals over time are insightful, but they cannot rule out that the relationship might be partly driven by other factors that we cannot observe. Even the studies linking loneliness and genetics can be subject to this <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="https://mru.org/dictionary-economics/omitted-variable-economics" target="_blank">omitted-variable bias</a>, because a genetic predisposition to loneliness may drive both loneliness and health outcomes.{ref}Two concrete papers that show this is a real concern are:<br>- Abdellaoui, A., Sanchez-Roige, S., Sealock, J., Treur, J. L., Dennis, J., Fontanillas, P., … & Baselmans, B. (2018). Phenome-wide investigation of health outcomes associated with genetic predisposition to loneliness. bioRxiv, 468835.<br>-Abdellaoui, A., Chen, H. Y., Willemsen, G., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Verweij, K. J., … & Cacioppo, J. T. (2019). Associations between loneliness and personality are mostly driven by a genetic association with neuroticism. Journal of personality, 87(2), 386-397.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>I could not find credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect.{ref}If you know of relevant studies that do provide such estimates, please let me know at [email protected].{/ref} But the fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect doesn't mean we should dismiss the available evidence. On the contrary – it would be great if we had evidence from randomized control trials that test positive interventions to reduce loneliness, to understand better if the ‘15 cigarettes per day’ comparison from the Surgeon General of the US is roughly correct, at least for the average person.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Having a better understanding of the magnitude of the effect is important, not only because loneliness is common, but also because it’s complex and unequally experienced by people around the world.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>As the chart above shows, there are large differences in self-reported loneliness across countries. We should understand how important these differences are for the distribution of health and well-being.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>Are we happier when we spend more time with others?</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 29774 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In 1938, a group of Harvard researchers decided to start a research program to track the lives of a group of young men, in what eventually became one of the longest and most famous longitudinal studies of its kind. The idea was to track the development of a group of teenage boys through periodic interviews and medical checkups, with the aim of understanding how their health and well-being evolved as they grew up.{ref}For historical context: The Harvard Graduate School of Education – the first to admit women – opened admissions to women in 1920; and the Harvard Medical School accepted its first female enrollees in 1945, even though the first woman applied in 1847. You can read more about the hard-earned gains for women at Harvard here: <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/" target="_blank">https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/</a>{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Today, more than 80 years later, it is one of the longest running research programmes in social science. It is called the <a href="https://www.adultdevelopmentstudy.org/" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">Harvard Study of Adult Development</a> and it is still running. The program started with 724 boys, and researchers continue to monitor today the health and well-being of those initial participants who are still alive, most in their late 90s.{ref}The original participants for this study came from two groups: a cohort of 268 men, 19 years of age, from the Harvard classes of 1939-1944; and a group of 456 caucasian men, ages 11-16, from underprivileged neighborhoods of Boston. Over the years the project evolved, enrolling more participants, and many of the children of the original participants are included in the study today.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This is a unique scientific exercise – most longitudinal studies do not last this long because too many participants drop out, researchers move on to other projects (or even die), or the funding dries up.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>So what have we learned from this unique study?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Robert Waldinger, the current director of the study, summarized – in what is now one of the <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://www.ted.com/talks/robert_waldinger_what_makes_a_good_life_lessons_from_the_longest_study_on_happiness?language=en" target="_blank">most viewed TED Talks to date</a> – the findings from decades of research. The main result, he concluded, is that social connections are one of the most important factors for people’s <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction" target="_blank">happiness</a> and <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta" target="_blank">health</a>. He said: “Those who kept warm relationships got to live longer and happier, and the loners often died earlier.”</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Here I take a closer look at the evidence and show more research that finds a consistent link between social connections and happiness. But before we get to the details, let me explain why I think this link is important.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>As most people can attest from personal experience, striving for happiness is not easy. In fact, the search for happiness can become a source of <em>unhappiness</em> – there are studies that show actively pursuing happiness <a href="https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">can end up decreasing it</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The data shows that <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#the-link-between-happiness-and-income" target="_blank" rel="noreferrer noopener">income and happiness are clearly related</a>; but we also know from surveys that people often <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/spc3.12386" target="_blank">overestimate the impact of income on happiness</a>. Social relations might be the missing link: In rich countries, where minimum material living conditions are <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="https://ourworldindata.org/extreme-poverty#historical-poverty-in-today-s-rich-countries" target="_blank">often satisfied</a>, people may struggle to become happier because they are targeting material rather than social goals.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>The cross-sectional correlation between happiness and friends</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/wvs.jsp" target="_blank">World Values Survey (WVS)</a> is a large cross-country research project that collects data from a series of representative national surveys. In the fourth wave (1999-2004) the WVS asked respondents hundreds of questions about their lives, including whether they were part of social or religious groups, how often they spent time with friends, and whether they felt happy with their life.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>By comparing self-reported happiness among those with and without frequent social interactions, we can get an idea of whether there is indeed a raw correlation between happiness and social relations across different societies.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the chart I show this comparison: The green points correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends at least once per month; while the blue dots correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends less often.{ref}The question on frequency of interaction with friends was asked in the context of time spent at work, clubs or places of worship. The options included ‘weekly’, ‘once or twice a month’, ‘only a few times a month’, and ‘not at all’. The options for happiness, on the other hand, included ‘very happy’, ‘quite happy’, ‘not very happy’ and ‘not at all happy’. In the chart I took this data, and I split the sample in two: I classified respondents as ‘often meeting with friends’ if they said they met with friends once a month or more frequently; and I classified them as ‘rarely meeting with friends’ if they said they met with friends ‘once a year’ or ‘not at all’. Then, for each of these two groups, I plotted the share who reported being ‘very happy’ or 'quite happy’.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This chart shows that in almost all countries, people who often spend time with their friends report to be happier than those who spend less time with friends.{ref}The estimates have of course a margin of error that varies with the survey size. The difference between the green and blue dots is statistically significant for about half of the countries (i.e. for about half of the countries in this chart, the 95% confidence intervals around the blue and green dots would not overlap). {/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:image {"align":"center","id":23997,"linkDestination":"custom"} --> <div class="wp-block-image"><figure class="aligncenter"><a href="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/07/happiness-and-friends-v3.png"><img src="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/07/happiness-and-friends-v3-e1564090552891.png" alt="" class="wp-image-23997"/></a></figure></div> <!-- /wp:image --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>The link between social relations and happiness over time</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart above gives a cross-sectional perspective – it’s just a snapshot that compares different people at a given point in time. What happens if we look at changes in social relations and happiness over time?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>There is a large academic literature in medicine and psychology that shows individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in life (you can read more about this in my <a rel="noopener noreferrer" href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being" target="_blank">post on social relations and health</a>); and similarly, there are also many studies that show that changes in social relations predict changes in happiness and life satisfaction.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>One of the research papers that draws on data from the famous Harvard Study of Adult Development, for example, looked at the experiences of 82 married participants and their spouses, and found that greater self-reported couple attachment predicted lower levels of depression and greater life satisfaction 2.5 years later.{ref}Waldinger, R. J., Cohen, S., Schulz, M. S., & Crowell, J. A. (2015). Security of attachment to spouses in late life: Concurrent and prospective links with cognitive and emotional well-being. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 516-529. Available online <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4579537/" target="_blank">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p> Other studies with larger population samples have also found a similar cross-temporal link: perceived social isolation predicts subsequent changes in depressive symptoms but not vice versa, and this holds after controlling for demographic variables and stress.{ref}See for example:<br>- Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and aging, 25(2), 453.<br>- Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and aging, 21(1), 140.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Searching for happiness is typically an intentional and active pursuit. Is it the case that people tend to become happier when they purposely decide to improve their social relations?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This is a tough empirical question to test; but <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0956797618761660" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">a recent study</a> found evidence pointing in this direction.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Using a large representative survey in Germany, researchers asked participants to report, in text format, ideas for how they could improve their life satisfaction. Based on these answers, the researchers then investigated which types of ideas predicted changes in life satisfaction one year later.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The researchers found that those who reported socially-engaged strategies (e.g. “I plan to spend more time with friends and family”) often reported improvements in life satisfaction one year later; while those who described other non-social active pursuits (e.g. “I plan to find a better job”) did not report increased life satisfaction.{ref}The researchers used an open question: “What could you do to ensure that you will be more satisfied in the future?”. There are of course many types of answers that people can provide to such an open-ended question, so the authors decided to classify each individual answer into one of two categories, depending on whether or not the respondents described a ‘socially engaged strategy’. This process of classifying answers could naturally raise questions: the authors could manipulate the subjective classification of answers to fit their favourite hypothesis. In order to insure themselves against this criticism, the researchers relied on three independent coders, and they stipulated the classification method and the expected results in a <a rel="noreferrer noopener" href="https://blogs.worldbank.org/impactevaluations/a-pre-analysis-plan-checklist" target="_blank">pre-analysis plan</a>. You find more details in the paper: Rohrer, J. M., Richter, D., Brümmer, M., Wagner, G. G., & Schmukle, S. C. (2018). Successfully striving for happiness: Socially engaged pursuits predict increases in life satisfaction. Psychological science, 29(8), 1291-1298.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Are we happier when we spend more time with others?</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>From decades of research we know that social relations predict mental well-being over time; and from a recent study we also know that people who actively decide to improve their social relations often report becoming happier. So yes, people are happier when they spend more time with friends.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Does this mean that if we have an exogenous shock to our social relations this will have a permanent negative effect on our happiness?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>We can’t really answer this with the available evidence. More research is needed to really understand the causal mechanisms that drive the link between happiness and social relations.{ref}The key point to keep in mind is that we are not talking about experimental evidence. People who frequently interact with friends, or people who say they want to engage more in ‘social strategies to improve their lives’ are likely different; and although the researchers do try to control for this, it is very hard to measure all the relevant characteristics that might help drive life satisfaction. Similarly, asking people about strategies to improve their lives is difficult, and although the qualitative strategy from the paper above is interesting, it’s still hard to draw accurate comparisons: it’s possible that ‘non-social’ strategies, for example, are more difficult or stressful to pursue; so part of the lower apparent impact that these strategies have on happiness might come from the fact that some people are trying but failing to find a better job.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Despite this, however, I think that we should take the available observational evidence seriously. In a way, the causal impact from a random shock is less interesting than the evidence from active strategies that people might take to improve their happiness. People who get divorced, for example, often report a short-term drop in life satisfaction; but over time they tend to recover and eventually end up being more satisfied with their life than shortly before they divorced (you can read more about this in our <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#life-satisfaction-through-life-events" target="_blank" rel="noreferrer noopener">entry on happiness and life satisfaction</a>).</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>It makes sense to consider the possibility that healthy social relationships are a key missing piece for human wellbeing. Among other things, this would help explain the paradoxical result from studies where actively pursuing happiness <a href="https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf" target="_blank" rel="noopener noreferrer">apparently ends up decreasing it</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>The importance of social networks for innovation and productivity</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 29782 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Economies today are increasingly built on ideas. Consider, for example, commercial aviation. After a flight, <em>Southwest Airlines </em>can unload and reload passengers to get the same airliner back in the air in about 10 minutes – this is half the time the same process took in the 1970s. As the economist John Cochrane <a href="https://review.chicagobooth.edu/economics/2019/article/what-success-rock-climbing-tells-us-about-economic-growth">explains</a>, this achievement is as much about ideas and know-how as it is about technology. <em>Southwest Airlines</em>, and many other carriers after them, have been able to expand their output because they now have <a href="https://www.npr.org/2015/06/28/418147961/the-man-who-saved-southwest-airlines-with-a-10-minute-idea">more efficient protocols</a> for ‘turning planes’ between flights.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In 2018 Paul Romer won the Nobel Prize in Economics “for integrating technological innovations into long-run macroeconomic analysis”. Romer first fleshed out this observation in his <a href="http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf">seminal 1990 paper</a>, titled “Endogenous Technological Change”. At the heart of his contributions was precisely this observation: the diffusion of ideas fosters economic and productivity growth.{ref}Romer’s seminal paper, with nearly 30,000 citations in Google Scholar is Romer, P. M. (1990). Endogenous technological change. Journal of Political Economy, 98 (5, Part 2), S71-S102. Available online <a href="http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf">here</a>. After this paper he wrote many other related articles elaborating on the same ideas. See, for example, Romer, P. M. (1992). Two strategies for economic development: using ideas and producing ideas. The World Bank Economic Review, 6(suppl_1), 63-91. Available online <a href="https://academic.oup.com/wber/article-abstract/6/suppl_1/63/1656015?redirectedFrom=fulltext">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Romer’s theory of “endogenous growth” – placing ideas at the heart of economic technologies – made economists ask entirely new questions about the drivers of growth. Where do ideas come from? How do ideas spread? </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Economists have asked these questions of the data, and their findings suggest social networks play a key role. Friendship and professional ties matter for economic growth because people tend to develop new ideas by interacting and learning from others who are close to them.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In a nutshell, social networks facilitate the diffusion of ideas across individuals and firms, and because of this, they play an important role in productivity growth. Here’s how this works.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Knowledge spillovers, social networks, and patent citations</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Economic theory suggests we should expect social connections to have an effect on productivity via “knowledge spillovers". </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The basic idea is that social connections make it easier for knowledge to travel farther and faster. Knowledge is different from many physical assets, in that it can be used by many people at the same time; so the diffusion of knowledge creates productivity gains that spillover through society. As Thomas Jefferson once said: <em>“He who receives an idea from me, receives instruction himself without lessening mine; as he who lights his taper at mine, receives light without darkening me”.</em>{ref}Letter to Isaac McPherson. August 13, 1813. Online <a href="https://en.wikisource.org/wiki/Letter_to_Isaac_McPherson_-_August_13,_1813">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>What is the empirical value of this theory? What do ‘knowledge spillovers’ actually look like in the real world? Do scientists and engineers really rely on social connections to disseminate their new ideas and inventions?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>To answer these questions economists have looked into historical data from <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patent-applications-per-million">patents</a>. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Patents are government licences used to register the intellectual property of new inventions, ranging <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patents-usa-by-category">from machines to medical drugs</a>. When a patent application is filed, inventors are required to include a list of citations, in which they describe how their technological development builds upon the results of earlier patents. These historical records of patent citations give us an overview of how knowledge flows – patent citations are the ‘paper trail’ left by ideas as they travel, evolve, and make new ideas possible.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>One of the first analyses of patent citations in the context of knowledge spillovers was done by Adam Jaffe, Manuel Trajtenberg and Rebecca Henderson, in <a href="https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813">a much-cited paper</a> published in the Quarterly Journal of Economics in 1993.{ref}Jaffe, A. B., Trajtenberg, M., & Henderson, R. (1993). Geographic localization of knowledge spillovers as evidenced by patent citations. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 108(3), 577-598. Available online <a href="https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Jaffe and coauthors studied, in particular, the geographic distribution of citation patterns in the US. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>If social connections did not matter for new ideas and growth, then we should expect that new inventions are just as likely to cite patents from far away as they are to cite the work of their neighbours. In contrast, Jaffe and coauthors found that new patents were disproportionately more likely to cite previous <em>local</em> patents. In other words, they found that patent citations were strongly geographically concentrated. According to their estimates, citations were three to four times as likely to come from the same state as the originating patent – this is consistent with the idea that social connections and proximity do indeed matter for innovation.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>After this initial study, several other papers have used patent citations to explore the determinants of knowledge spillovers, with a broader perspective on ‘proximity’. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Ajay Agrawal, Devesh Kapur and John McHale used the ethnicity of researchers as a proxy for social connectedness, and <a href="http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.515.3876&rep=rep1&type=pdf">found</a> that co-location and co-ethnicity both increase the probability of knowledge flow between inventors. Co-location increases the probability of a knowledge flow by 24% (assuming non-co-ethnic inventors) and co-ethnicity increases the probability by 14% (assuming non-co-located inventors). Again, these large concentration patterns seem consistent with the idea that proximity and social networks matter for innovation.{ref} Agrawal, A., Kapur, D., & McHale, J. (2008). How do spatial and social proximity influence knowledge flows? Evidence from patent data. Journal of Urban Economics, 64(2), 258-269.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Disentangling geographic proximity and social interactions</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The correlation between proximity and patent citations partly captures the effect that local factors may have on innovation (e.g. geography, regulation or infrastructure around industry hubs where similar workers and firms concentrate). However, the evidence shows that social connections matter for the transmission of knowledge even after we control for these other factors.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In <a href="https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259">a paper</a> published in 2018 in the <em>Journal of Economic Perspectives</em>, Michael Bailey, Rachel Cao, Theresa Kuchler, Johannes Stroebel, and Arlene Wong combined patent records with Facebook data from the US, and found that social connectedness between US counties (measured by the frequency of Facebook ties between counties) predicts the probability of cross-county patent citations. This is true, even after accounting for the patents’ technology class and geographic distance between counties.{ref} The full reference of the paper is: Bailey, M., Cao, R., Kuchler, T., Stroebel, J., & Wong, A. (2018). Social connectedness: measurement, determinants, and effects. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32(3), 259-80. Available online <a href="https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This means that the probability that an inventor builds on the work of another inventor in a different part of the country can be partly explained by how well-connected their social backgrounds are, even after controlling for geographic distance and other demographic factors.{ref}To be more precise, Bailey and co-authors find that the effect of doubling social connectedness on the probability of citation remains significant and large, at 4.9 percent, even after controlling for geographic distance. When they add controls for cross-country differences on socioeconomic indicators (income, education, ethnicity, etc.) they actually find that the estimated relationship between social connectedness and patent citation increases somewhat as a result of these further controls. This finding suggests that the relationship between geographic distance and the probability of patent citation, viewed in isolation, may be partially capturing effects of information flows associated with social connectedness.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The implications of this result are of great consequence in the age of the internet. The development of new communication-enabling technologies, such as <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media">the rise of social media</a>, have opened new possibilities for creating and maintaining social connections, and this is likely to have had a positive effect on the transmission of knowledge.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Social connections and (material) well-being</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Globally, the total economic output per person today is <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&tab=chart&year=1950&time=1870..latest&country=OWID_WRL">4.4-times larger</a> than in 1950 (even after accounting for inflation). This was achieved over a period during which people tended towards working <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/annual-working-hours-per-worker?tab=chart&stackMode=absolute&time=1950..2017&country=BRA~COL~FRA~DEU~NLD~GBR~USA&region=World" data-type="URL" data-id="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/annual-working-hours-per-worker?tab=chart&stackMode=absolute&time=1950..2017&country=BRA~COL~FRA~DEU~NLD~GBR~USA&region=World">fewer hours</a>. We now produce more whilst working less.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>As our interactive chart here shows, this historical achievement is underpinned by productivity growth: in many countries, workers produce much higher output per hour today than in 1950.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This is partly because of new machines and technologies, but also because we have new shared knowledge about how to work more efficiently.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The research on social connections and innovation suggests that one important way to improve material living standards is to invest in digital communication technologies. This lowers the costs of creating and maintaining personal and professional ties, which facilitates the diffusion of ideas and knowledge, creating positive productivity spillovers.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <figure><iframe src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/labor-productivity-per-hour-PennWorldTable?time=1950..2017&country=BRA+DEU+IND+JPN+KOR+GBR+USA"></iframe></figure> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:owid/additional-information --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>Technical Note:</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Social networks are diverse and complex, so talking about “social connectedness” is actually a simplification that deserves some discussion. Researchers studying social networks have shown that the flow of information and knowledge through people is not only a function of “connectedness” as measured by the number of links people have, but also a function of the specific structures of the networks (e.g. the extent to which social ties are tightly clustered). </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>For the diffusion of information, the concept of ‘homophily’, for example, is key. <br>Homophily is the tendency of agents to associate with other agents who have similar characteristics. This manifests itself in many ways – for example, in the fact that friendship ties are typically strongly related to ethnicity. If one is interested in how information spreads through social learning, patterns such as homophily or social segregation are important to understand. For example, in the labor market, homophily isolates workers of one ethnicity from workers of another ethnicity, which then limits the extent to which individuals in one group hear about openings and opportunities known to the other group.<br>Social networks research in this area shows that, in general, as homophily increases, the propensity for a diffusion to gain hold within a particular group rises, sometimes at the expense of the speed and extent of diffusion throughout the entire population. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Similarly, diversity may play a role. Observational studies <a href="https://science.sciencemag.org/content/328/5981/1029">show</a> that the diversity of individuals’ relationships is strongly correlated with the economic development of communities.<br>You can read more about this in Jackson, M. O., Rogers, B. W., & Zenou, Y. (2017). The economic consequences of social-network structure. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 49-95. Available online <a href="https://www.econstor.eu/bitstream/10419/175053/1/Wp1116.pdf">here</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- /wp:owid/additional-information --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 29786 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, West German families that had relatives in East Germany saw an increase in their income, and this growth in incomes was <a href="https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf">significantly higher</a> than that of comparable West German families without such family ties to the East. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Why did this happen? Are personal and family ties really so important to our economic outcomes that they result in systematic differences in income growth?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Researchers have studied this question and have found empirical support for the idea that social connections do, indeed, have large effects on economic outcomes. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>A natural experiment: Friends and incomes after the fall of the Berlin Wall</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Measuring the impact of personal relations on economic outcomes is difficult. This is because these two aspects of our life are often closely intertwined – social ties facilitate economic interactions, but at the same time people with whom we interact for economic reasons end up becoming acquaintances or even friends. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>To overcome this difficulty, the economists Konrad Burchardi and Tarek Hassan studied a historical event that suddenly opened up new economic interactions, where previously only non-economic social connections were possible: The fall of the Berlin Wall.{ref}Konrad B. Burchardi and Tarek A. Hassan. "The economic impact of social ties: Evidence from German reunification." The Quarterly Journal of Economics 128.3 (2013): 1219-1271. Available online <a href="https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>For decades private economic exchange between East and West Germany was virtually impossible, but at the same time people on both sides maintained social ties for non-economic reasons. After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, trade and collaboration between the two Germanies suddenly became feasible. Burchardi and Hassan studied the impact that this sudden change had on household incomes.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989 there was <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&time=1500..2018&country=~DEU">growth in incomes</a> across Germany as a whole; but the interesting finding from Burchardi and Hassan is that income growth for households in West Germany who had ties to at least one relative in the East was much higher – six percentage points higher – than that of comparable households without such ties.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Burchardi and Hassan argue that West German households that had ties to East Germany had a comparative advantage in seizing the new economic opportunities in the East. Having personal relationships with East Germans gave them access to valuable economic information – information regarding local demand conditions, and about the quality of East German assets that were offered to investors.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>These gains from social connections actually added up at the regional level. West German regions that had a higher concentration of households with social ties to the East, enjoyed substantially higher growth in incomes in the early 1990s. A one standard deviation rise in the share of households with social ties to East Germany in 1989 was associated with a 4.6 percentage point rise in income per capita over six years.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Personal relations and job search: Evidence from Europe</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The unique situation of the German reunification shows that social ties that are initially unrelated to economic interests can become important for economic success.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The key mechanism is that personal connections allow us to seize new economic opportunities. This may sound abstract, but in fact most of us have experienced this in our own lives. Perhaps the best example is job searching: personal contacts and connections are one of the most common ways for people to find a job.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the book <em>The Human Network</em>, the economist Matthew Jackson draws on decades of research to help explain how social connections affect power, beliefs and behaviors. With regard to job networks and social capital, he writes: “<em>If you have ever struggled to find a job, you are not alone. Without well-connected friends or family it is hard to make it in any industry [...]. Finding a job without having some personal connection to someone already employed at a firm is more the exception than the rule.” </em>{ref}Pages 239-241 in Jackson, M. O. (2019). <em>The Human Network: How Your Social Position Determines Your Power, Beliefs, and Behaviors</em>. Pantheon.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Our interactive chart here shows evidence for Jackson’s claim using data from European countries. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart shows the distribution of job search methods by country. Data for Spain is shown by default, but you can explore data for different countries using the option ‘Change country’.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In Spain, 84% of the unemployed rely on contacts – friends, relatives, trade unions – to find a job. It is the most common job-search method. Direct applications and ads rank below, on places two and three.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>If you plot the same data for other countries you’ll see that personal contacts are a very important job-search method everywhere in Europe, but there is some variation across countries. At the lowest end of the spectrum, in Belgium, about 38% of people rely on contacts to find a job; at the other end, in Greece and Latvia, 90% of job seekers rely on this method.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><em>(NB. </em><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-friends-relatives-trade-unions-in-job-search?tab=map"><em>This interactive map</em></a><em> shows the same data, but focusing specifically on the proportion of job seekers relying on contacts for job search across Europe.)</em></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <figure><iframe src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-specific-search-methods"></iframe></figure> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Friends and relatives remain a popular method for finding a job, even in the age of digital job search platforms </h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart showing job search methods across Europe relies on data from the European Labor Force Survey, 2006-2008. We have not found more recent data that allows cross-country comparisons, but surveys from individual high-income countries show that this remains relevant today, even after the expansion of online job search engines, and <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media">the rise of social media platforms</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the US, data from the Pew Research Center shows that the internet is the most important resource for job search, but 66% of survey respondents still say they relied on connections with close friends or family in their most recent search for a job. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>These two methods are of course not mutually exclusive, and the boundaries between ‘internet’ and ‘personal contact’ are becoming less clear as job search platforms become more integrated with social media.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <figure><iframe src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/job-seeker-job-search-pew"></iframe></figure> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Personal relations matter for trade</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Personal relations can also provide information and social collateral that can be valuable for economic interactions across national borders.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The political scientist Anna Lee Saxenian <a href="https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf">analyzed</a> the biographies of Chinese and Indian engineers who migrated to California in the 1970s. She found that as these skilled immigrants excelled in their personal careers, they started leveraging their social ties to relatives and friends in their home regions. This allowed them to outsource important operations for a large fraction of Silicon Valley firms.{ref}Saxenian, A. (2000). Silicon Valley’s new immigrant entrepreneurs. UC San DiegoWorking Papers. The Center for Comparative Immigration Studies. Available online <a href="https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Saxenian argues that by connecting Silicon Valley firms to low-cost, high-quality labor in India and China, they became instrumental for the success of Silicon Valley and crucial in the emergence of their home regions as major hubs of the global IT services industry.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Personal relations matter for <em>collective</em> economic outcomes</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In some situations personal connections may facilitate economic transactions for one person (or group of people) at the expense of another one. This is what many people have in mind when they think about job referrals. If there is only one job vacancy, giving the job to a friend means not giving the job to someone else. If social connections become a vehicle for favouritism, this can lead to inefficiencies.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In many contexts, however, the opposite is true. Social connections often have a net-positive effect on aggregate economic outcomes because much of the impact of social connections on economic transactions is not zero-sum – for example, when social connections reduce information frictions and allow people to learn from each other. As we discuss in a <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-networks-innovation-and-productivity">companion post</a>, there is empirical evidence showing these knowledge spillovers are substantial.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Social networks can affect economic outcomes via agglomeration effects</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The concentration of firms and workers in ‘industry hubs’ is partly explained by the fact that social connections create economic externalities: Agglomeration leads to denser social connections, and this has positive spillover effects on productivity.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Again the fall of the Berlin Wall offers clear evidence of this.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>When Berlin was split, economic activity in West Berlin shifted away from the historic business district, known as ‘Mitte’. As a consequence property prices there fell relative to the new economic areas that emerged further away from the Wall. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>When the Wall came down the distribution of economic activity within the city changed quickly once again: The old business district began to re-emerge and economic activity shifted back to the historic center. This is the conclusion of a careful econometric study, published by Gabriel Ahlfeldt, Stephen Redding, Daniel Sturm and Nikolaus Wolf in the journal <em>Econometrica</em>.{ref} Ahlfeldt, G. M., Redding, S. J., Sturm, D. M., & Wolf, N. (2015). The economics of density: Evidence from the Berlin Wall. Econometrica, 83(6), 2127-2189. Online <a href="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.3982/ECTA10876">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The three maps show the evolution of land prices in Berlin. These maps, from Ahlfeldt and co-authors, illustrate graphically the impact that the Wall had on the geography of economic activity. The vertical scale measures relative land prices; this means higher elevation, highlighted with darker shades, denotes higher prices.{ref} The authors use the same vertical scale for each figure, and land prices are normalized to have a mean of 1 in each year, so the levels of the land price surfaces in each map are comparable.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The map on the left shows that in 1936 there are two areas with very high land values in West Berlin and both are close to the center (they are both part of a concentric ring around the pre-war central business district, located in the middle).</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In 1986, we see smaller peak values; in fact, we see that the pre-war peak closest to the Wall is entirely eliminated following division, as this area ceased to be an important center of commercial and retail activity. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Finally, in 2006, we see again higher peaks; and we see that the area closest to the Wall is reemerging as a center of high land values. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This evolution is a great example of the economics of density: The areas in West Berlin close to the old central business district became less productive during the division but regained their productivity and attractiveness again soon after reunification. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Knowledge, public resources, and opportunities for trade and collaboration that had been inaccessible for decades, suddenly became available after the fall of the Wall.{ref} Ahlfeldt and coauthors disentangle agglomeration effects from other forces that took place at the same time. In particular, they account for the fact that the Berlin Wall had an impact both on agglomeration forces (i.e. “production externalities” determined by the surrounding density of workers); as well as on "production fundamentals" (i.e. the physical characteristics of the different neighborhoods where workers and firms concentrate).{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:image {"id":27847} --> <figure class="wp-block-image"><img src="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/11/berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015-800x389.png" alt="" class="wp-image-27847"/></figure> <!-- /wp:image --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Social connections matter hugely for economic outcomes. Personal relations, even those that we maintain for non-economic reasons, often give us access to information and provide us with social collateral for economic transactions – from buying a house to getting a job. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This can have positive consequences that extend beyond the individuals directly involved in the transactions. When personal and professional ties facilitate the exchange of information and knowledge, this can bring benefits to society as a whole. The implication from this is that social connections are not only important because they <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being">affect our emotional well-being</a>, but also because, as we show here, they affect our <em>material</em> well-being too.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading --> <h2>Loneliness, solitude and social isolation</h2> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>Living alone is becoming increasingly common around the world</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 29791 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the US, the share of adults who live alone nearly doubled over the last 50 years. This is not only happening in the US: single-person households have become increasingly common in many countries across the world, from Angola to Japan.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Historical records show that this ‘rise of living alone’ started in early-industrialized countries over a century ago, accelerating around 1950. In countries such as Norway and Sweden, single-person households were rare a century ago, but today they account for nearly half of all households. In some cities they are already the majority.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Surveys and census data from recent decades show that people are more likely to live alone in rich countries, and the prevalence of single-person households is unprecedented historically. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Social connections – including contact with friends and family – are <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being">important</a> for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as single-person households become more common, there will be new challenges to connect and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where welfare states are weaker.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>But it’s important to keep things in perspective. It’s unhelpful to compare the rise of living alone with a ‘loneliness epidemic’, which is what newspaper articles often write in <a href="https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA">alarming headlines</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Loneliness and solitude are <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone">not the same</a>, and the evidence suggests that self-reported loneliness has <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic"><em>not</em> been growing</a> in recent decades.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Historical perspective on people living alone: Evidence from rich countries </h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Historical records of inhabitants across villages and cities in today’s rich countries give us insights into how uncommon it was for people to live alone in the past.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart here, adapted from <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true">a paper</a> by the historian Keith Snell, shows estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data. Each dot corresponds to an estimate for one settlement in Europe, North America, Japan or Britain.{ref}Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28. Online <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The share of one-person households remained fairly steady between the early modern period and through the 19th century – typically below 10%. Then growth started in the twentieth century, accelerating in the 1960s.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The current prevalence of one-person households is unprecedented historically. The highest point recorded in this chart corresponds to Stockholm, in 2012, where 60% of households consist of one person.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:image {"id":28961,"sizeSlug":"large"} --> <figure class="wp-block-image size-large"><img src="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/historical-one-person-households-652x550.png" alt="" class="wp-image-28961"/></figure> <!-- /wp:image --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>The rise of one-person households across the world</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>For recent decades, census data can be combined with data from large cross-country surveys, to provide a global perspective on the proportion of households with only one member (i.e. the proportion of single-person households). This gives us a proxy for the prevalence of solitary living arrangements.{ref}The percentage of single-person households is a proxy for ‘solitary living’. However, it’s important to emphasise that these two things are not really the same. Single-person households include those where a person lives alone in an individual housing unit, but they also include people who live independently as lodgers in a separate room within a larger housing unit with other occupant. So technically, there are some people who live in a ‘single-person household’, but they don’t really live alone.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>We produced this chart combining individual reports from statistical country offices, cross-country surveys such as the <a href="https://dhsprogram.com/"><em>Demographic and Health Surveys</em></a>, and estimates published in the EU’s <em><a href="https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat">Eurostat</a></em>, the UN’s <a href="https://unstats.un.org/unsd/demographic-social/products/dyb/">Demographic Year Books</a>, and the <a href="https://figshare.com/articles/German_Time_Series_Dataset_1834_2012/1450809/1">Deutschland in Daten</a> dataset. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart shows that the trend of rising single-person households extends across all world regions. There are large differences between countries – from more than 40% in northern European countries to 1% in low-income Asian countries.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><em>(NB. For the US and Canada there are long-run time series from census data that let us directly track the share of people who live alone. This is shown in </em><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/adults-living-alone"><em>this other chart</em></a><em>, where you can see the same trend.) </em><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households"></a></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <iframe src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households" style="width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;"></iframe> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Living arrangements and prosperity</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>National income per capita and the share of one-person households are strongly correlated: As the chart here shows, people are more likely to live alone in rich countries.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In this interactive chart you can move the slider to see changes over time. This reveals that the rise of single-person households tends to be larger in countries where GDP per capita has grown more. <em>(NB. You can also see the correlation over time in </em><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?stackMode=relative&time=1990..2016"><em>this other</em></a><em> scatter plot comparing average growth in GDP vs average growth in one-person households).</em></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>These correlations are partly due to the fact that people who can afford to, often choose to live alone. Indeed, rising incomes in many countries are likely part of the reason why people are more likely to live alone today than in the past. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>But there must be more to it since even at the same level of incomes there are clear differences between regions. In particular, Asian countries have systematically fewer one-person households than African countries with comparable GDP levels. Ghana and Pakistan, for example, have similar GDP per capita, but in Pakistan one-person households are extremely rare, while in Ghana they are common (about 1 in 4). This suggests culture and country-specific factors also play an important role.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Additionally, there are other non-cultural country-specific factors that are likely to play a role. In particular, rich countries often have more extensive social support networks, so people in these countries find it easier to take risks. Living alone is more risky in poorer countries, because there’s often less supply of services and infrastructure to support more solitary living arrangements. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>And finally, it’s also likely that some of the causality runs in the opposite direction. It’s not only that incomes, culture or welfare states enable people to live alone, but also that for many workers attaining higher incomes in today’s economy often demands changes in living arrangements. Migration<a href="https://ourworldindata.org/urbanization"> from rural to urban areas</a> is the prime example.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <iframe src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita" style="width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;"></iframe> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Is the rise of one-person households a problem? </h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Social connections – including contact with friends and family – are <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being">important</a> for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as the ‘rise of living alone’ continues, there will be new challenges to connect people and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where communication technologies are <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/internet">less developed</a> and welfare states are <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/government-spending#high-income-countries-spend-more-on-social-protection-than-low-income-countries">weaker</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>But, it’s also important to keep in mind that living alone is <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone">not the same</a> as feeling lonely. There’s evidence that living alone is, by itself, a poor predictor of loneliness. Self-reported loneliness has <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic">not been growing </a>in recent decades, and in fact, the countries where people are most likely to say they have support from family and friends, are the same countries – in Scandinavia – where a large fraction of the population lives alone.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Incomes and freedom of choice are not the only drivers of the ‘rise of living alone’; but it would be remiss to ignore they do contribute to this trend.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><a href="https://ourworldindata.org/economic-growth">Higher incomes</a>, <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/structural-transformation-and-deindustrialization-evidence-from-todays-rich-countries">economic transitions</a> that enable migration from agriculture in rural areas into manufacturing and services in cities, and <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/female-labor-supply">rising female participation in labor markets</a> all play a role. People are more likely to live alone today than in the past partly because they are increasingly able to do so.<br></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>Loneliness and aloneness are not the same</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 29797 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>There is a popular <a href="https://www.vice.com/sv/article/mvn7eb/the-swedish-theory-of-love-or-why-we-swedes-are-so-lonely-234">perception</a> that countries in Northern Europe are heavily individualistic and because of this people in these societies tend to be much lonelier. The data, however, does not support this claim.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>What is true is that in countries such as Denmark and Switzerland, it is <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE">very common</a> for people to live alone. But contrary to what many believe, this does not translate into higher levels of self-reported loneliness.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Below we discuss the data in more detail, and show that this can be partly explained by the fact that loneliness and aloneness are just not the same. Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it’s important not to conflate them.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Self-reported loneliness</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Loneliness describes a <em>subjective</em> feeling; this is conceptually distinct from <em>objective</em> physical isolation.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the chart here we show estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness among older adults. The data comes from various surveys asking people directly whether they often experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. <em>“I have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with”</em>).</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The differences in the prevalence of loneliness across countries are very large. At the bottom of the list, Denmark, Switzerland, Sweden and the US all have rates below 30%, while at the top of the list Greece, Israel and Italy all have rates of close to or above 50%.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The data is available only for fifteen countries, but this sample does not suggest that <a href="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Individualistic_culture">'rich individualistic societies'</a> are lonelier than others. In fact, at the very bottom are two countries in which the share of people living alone is among the <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE">highest</a> in the world.<a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults"></a></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <iframe src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults" style="width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;"></iframe> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Perceptions of social support</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Similar to loneliness, we can measure perceptions of social support by asking people directly. This is what the polling organization <em>Gallup</em> did in their flagship <a href="https://www.gallup.com/analytics/232838/world-poll.aspx"><em>World Poll</em></a> survey. Specifically, they asked: <em>“If you were in trouble, do you have relatives or friends you can count on to help you whenever you need them, or not?”</em></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart here presents the results from this survey, plotting the share of people who responded “yes” to this question.{ref} The data for this chart comes from a table published in the appendix of the OECD report “How's Life? 2017 Measuring Well-Being”, available online <a href="https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Differences across countries are not very large. The lowest and highest average levels of support corresponds to Mexico and Iceland, at 80% and 98% respectively. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The second point that stands out is that, again, there’s no support for the claim that richer countries that are considered to be more individualistic (e.g. North European countries) have lower levels of family and friendship support. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:html --> <iframe src="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/people-who-report-having-friends-or-relatives-they-can-count-on" style="width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;"></iframe> <!-- /wp:html --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Distinguishing between aloneness and loneliness</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Are people who live alone more likely to say they feel lonely?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The researchers Caitlin Coyle and Elizabeth Dugan explored this question using data from the<em> Health and Retirement Study</em> in the US. In their <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275">analysis</a>, covering almost 12,000 respondents over the years 2006-2008, they found that loneliness and social isolation are not highly correlated at the individual level.{ref}Coyle, C. E., & Dugan, E. (2012). Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. Journal of aging and health, 24(8), 1346-1363. Online <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>To be precise, Coyle and Dugan found a correlation coefficient of 0.2. That means the correlation is positive, and it is statistically larger than zero; but it’s not very large in absolute terms. Their study does not provide the raw data, but in general, a scatter plot with correlation 0.2 is pretty much just a <a href="https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Correlation_and_dependence">cloud of dots</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This means you cannot predict much of the variability in loneliness from observed differences in living arrangements. Some people who live alone are lonely, but many are not. Concluding that someone who lives alone must feel lonely is very often wrong.{ref}There’s similar evidence from other countries. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the <em>Community Life Survey</em>, in which they ask about living arrangements and loneliness. This survey finds that while the reported frequency of loneliness is certainly higher among those who live alone, the correlation is not higher that of other ‘risk factors’ such as disability, age and marital status. You find more information in the ONS report <a href="https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Being alone and feeling lonely are not the same</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>People do not seem to be more lonely in societies that are <a href="https://www.hofstede-insights.com/country-comparison/finland,norway,sweden/">traditionally labeled</a> as 'individualistic'.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>What is true is that in these societies it is <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE">particularly common</a> for people to live alone. But being alone and feeling lonely don’t always go hand in hand. Many people feel lonely even if they are not physically isolated; and many people who are physically isolated do not feel lonely. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The aggregate statistics confirm this. Surveys that ask people about living arrangements, time use, and feelings of loneliness, find that solitude, by itself, does not predict feelings of loneliness. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The fact that people in individualistic countries are not more likely to feel lonely may of course capture differences in expectations regarding social relations. But to the extent that loneliness is a subjective experience, these cross-country differences are still important to understand well-being.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it’s important not to conflate them.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>Changes in loneliness over time: Is there a loneliness epidemic?</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 29805 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The media seems to have agreed that rich countries are experiencing a ‘loneliness epidemic’. There are literally <a href="https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA">thousands</a> of newspaper articles that use this exact expression.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>What is the evidence for this? The word ‘epidemic’ suggests that things are getting much worse and loneliness is increasing rapidly. But does the data in fact show that societies are becoming lonelier?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Despite the popularity of the claim, there is surprisingly no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>It is <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone">true</a> that more people are living alone around the world. But loneliness and aloneness are not the same. As we explain in a <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone">companion post</a>, spending time alone is not a good predictor of whether people feel lonely, or have weaker social support.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>As we explain later, today’s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today’s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than older adults in the past. Surveys covering older adults in other rich countries, including Finland, Germany, England and Sweden, point in the same direction – it’s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations in these countries.{ref} See the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online <a href="https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Social connections – including contact with friends and family – are important for our <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being">health and emotional welfare</a>, as well as for our <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/personal-relations-econ-outcomes">material well-being</a>. Loneliness is indeed an important problem, but it’s crucial to have a nuanced conversation based on facts. The headlines that claim we are witnessing a ‘loneliness epidemic’ are wrong and unhelpful.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Are young people more lonely than older adults?</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>One statistic that is often used to argue that loneliness is increasing, is that young people today are lonelier than older adults. This begs two questions: (i) Is it true that younger people are lonelier, and (ii) does this show that loneliness is increasing?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Let’s start with the first question. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the <a href="https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/community-life-survey-2018-19">Community Life Survey</a>, in which they ask people how often they feel lonely. In the bar chart here we show a breakdown of self-reported loneliness by age group.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>According to this data, those aged 16 to 24 are the group most likely to report feeling lonely, with 10% feeling lonely “often or always”. In contrast, those aged 65 years and older are the group least likely to report feeling lonely, with 3% feeling lonely “often or always”. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Many people tend to associate loneliness with older age, so this pattern might seem surprising. But surveys from several other rich countries have found the same. In <a href="http://archive.stats.govt.nz/browse_for_stats/people_and_communities/older_people/loneliness-in-nz-2010-NZGSS/loneliness-in-nz.aspx">New Zealand</a>, <a href="http://files.kff.org/attachment/Report-Loneliness-and-Social-Isolation-in-the-United-States-the-United-Kingdom-and-Japan-An-International-Survey">Japan</a> and the <a href="https://www.multivu.com/players/English/8294451-cigna-us-loneliness-survey/docs/IndexReport_1524069371598-173525450.pdf">US</a>, young adults also report feeling lonely more often than older adults.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>So, yes, in rich countries we find that younger people are more likely to report feeling lonely.{ref}The emphasis on ‘rich countries’ is important. A study exploring self-reported loneliness across nine former Soviet countries, found that older adults report higher levels of loneliness. This shows it's wrong to generalize from the available data, which is primarily from rich countries. For more details see: Stickley, A., Koyanagi, A., Roberts, B., Richardson, E., Abbott, P., Tumanov, S., & McKee, M. (2013). Loneliness: its correlates and association with health behaviours and outcomes in nine countries of the former Soviet Union. PloS one, 8(7), e67978. Online <a href="https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>What about the second question? Does this mean that loneliness is increasing? </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Here, the answer is ‘no’. Cross-sectional comparisons are not informative about changes <em>over time</em>, because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle. To be able to say something meaningful about changes in loneliness over time, we need to distinguish between changes for <em>individuals</em> over time (do people become lonelier as they get older?) and changes across generations (are people of the same age lonelier today than in the past?).</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Let’s dig deeper and explore these questions separately. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:image {"id":29000,"sizeSlug":"large"} --> <figure class="wp-block-image size-large"><img src="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-731x550.png" alt="" class="wp-image-29000"/></figure> <!-- /wp:image --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Do we become lonelier as we get older?</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>To understand how loneliness changes across our life cycle, we need loneliness data from surveys that track the same individuals over time, up until old age. In a <a href="https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download">study</a> published in the journal <em>Psychology and Aging</em>, Louise Hawkley and co-authors examine two such surveys, with data for adults older than 50 years in the US.{ref} Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online <a href="https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>They found that after age 50 – which is the earliest age of participants in their study – loneliness tended to decrease, until about 75, after which it began to increase again.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The authors explain in their paper that the increase in loneliness after 75 was explained by a decline in health and the loss of a spouse or partner. When adjusting for these factors, they found that loneliness continued declining into ‘oldest old age’.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This shows that there are two forces at play. On the one hand, there seems to be a direct relationship between age and loneliness, whereby loneliness <em>decreases</em> with age as our social expectations adapt, and we become <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1207/S15324834BASP2304_2?casa_token=885gubc-kPAAAAAA:gP8oVDQ-085Ftuh1-pvmG4Ms4-O58KDka4hjgK5Nq7RjS3ZsESymhhPo_owFbaf5MufXSx2LvFc0oA">more selective</a> about relating with contacts who bring positive emotions. On the other hand, there seems to be an indirect association pushing in the opposite direction, whereby loneliness <em>increases</em> with age, because our health deteriorates and we lose relatives and friends. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In our middle age the direct effect dominates, but once we enter advanced old age, the negative indirect effect starts dominating.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This complex relationship between age and loneliness shows why comparing old and young people at a given point in time is misleading. Cross-sectional comparisons are just not informative about the evolution of loneliness over time, because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Loneliness trends over time: Are people lonelier today than in the past?</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the ‘loneliness epidemic’ narrative, it is often implied that if we compare two individuals of the same age – one today and another one a generation ago – we would find that the one today is more likely to feel lonely. This is based on the idea that there have been societal changes – such as the <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone">rise of living alone</a> – that make newer generations more likely to feel lonely.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In their <a href="https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download">study</a>, Louise Hawkley and co-authors searched for evidence of these ‘cohort trends’ in the US, but didn’t find any. There was very little difference in self-reported loneliness of people born in different generations. Those that were born in 1920-1947 experienced the same changes of loneliness throughout their lives as those born in 1948-1965. It’s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>An <a href="https://unherd.com/2019/05/is-the-epidemic-of-loneliness-fake-news/">article</a> by Tom Chivers explains this result from the study very well: <em>“[Hawkley and co-authors] found that “newer” old people (baby boomers born 1948-1965) are no more likely to think of themselves as lonely than “older” old people (born 1920-1947), and that older people have not become more likely to think of themselves as lonely in the last decade (2005 – 2016)... The average older person appears to be no more likely to be lonely than they were a decade ago.”</em></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>There are studies with data from other rich countries pointing in the same direction. In Sweden, repeated cross-sectional surveys with adults aged 85, 90, and 95 years old, found no increase in loneliness over a ten-year interval.{ref} Nyqvist, F., Cattan, M., Conradsson, M., Näsman, M., & Gustafsson, Y. (2017). Prevalence of loneliness over ten years among the oldest old. Scandinavian journal of public health, 45(4), 411-418. Available online <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1403494817697511">here</a>. For similar results in other countries, including Finland, Germany and England, see the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online <a href="https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>These studies find no evidence of cohort effects across older adults. What about evidence for adolescents?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the aptly titled <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789">paper</a> “Rethinking ‘Generation Me’: A Study of Cohort Effects from 1976-2006”, the psychologists Kali Trzesniewski and Brent Donnellan used repeated cohort surveys to explore whether successive groups of high-school graduates were becoming more lonely in the US. They also found no evidence of cohort trends. Newer generations of high-school seniors were not more likely to report feelings of loneliness than earlier generations.{ref} Trzesniewski and Donnellan relied on data from the <a href="http://www.monitoringthefuture.org">Monitoring the Future Project</a>, an ongoing survey of young Americans. Their analysis extends beyond loneliness. The paper abstract explains the results as follows: <em>“Social commentators have argued that changes over the last decades have coalesced to create a relatively unique generation of young people. However, using large samples of U.S. high-school seniors from 1976 to 2006 (Total N = 477,380), we found little evidence of meaningful change in egotism, self-enhancement, individualism, self-esteem, locus of control, hopelessness, happiness, life satisfaction, loneliness, antisocial behavior, time spent working or watching television, political activity, the importance of religion, and the importance of social status over the last 30 years.” </em>The full reference of the paper is: Trzesniewski, K. H., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Rethinking “Generation Me”: A study of cohort effects from 1976-2006. Perspectives on psychological science, 5(1), 58-75. Available online <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The psychologists Matthew Clark, Natalie Loxton, and Stephanie Tobin <a href="https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0146167214557007">replicated</a> this analysis, using the same survey, but focusing on all age groups, not only high-school seniors. The chart here shows their results. They found no signs of increasing loneliness across all age groups. In fact, they found a very small but statistically significant <em>decline in loneliness </em>for high school students in the US.{ref} To measure loneliness, the project used a six-item questionnaire, where participants used a 1 (disagree) to 5 (agree) scale to respond to the following statements: ‘‘<em>A lot of times I feel lonely</em>,’’ ‘‘<em>There is always someone I can turn to if I need help (reverse scored)</em>,’’ ‘‘<em>I often feel left out of things</em>,’’ ‘‘<em>There is usually someone I can talk to if I need to (reverse scored)</em>,’’ ‘‘<em>I often wish I had more good friends</em>,’’ and ‘<em>‘I usually have a few good friends around that I can get together with (reverse scored).</em>’’ The project began collecting loneliness data for Grade 12 students in 1977, and data for Grades 8 and 10 in 1991. {/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p><em>(NB. The vertical axis in this chart is truncated, following the presentation in the original paper. The truncated axis is helpful to highlight the trend; but the takeaway is that the changes in levels are extremely small, so the trend is effectively flat in absolute terms, even if the slope is statistically different from zero).</em></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:image {"id":29001,"sizeSlug":"large"} --> <figure class="wp-block-image size-large"><img src="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-786x550.png" alt="" class="wp-image-29001"/></figure> <!-- /wp:image --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Loneliness deserves attention, but headlines that claim we are witnessing a ‘loneliness epidemic’ are not true, and are actually unhelpful</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The magazine <em>The Economist </em>wrote <a href="https://www.economist.com/international/2018/09/01/loneliness-is-a-serious-public-health-problem">an article</a> in 2018 with the title “Loneliness is a serious public-health problem”. In one key paragraph, the article reads: “Historical data about loneliness are scant. But isolation does seem to be increasing, so loneliness may be too.” </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The data we have found shows that this reasoning is actually incorrect. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Surveys from rich countries do not suggest there has been an increase in loneliness over time. Today’s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today’s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than did adults of their age in the past.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>That’s of course not to say we should not pay attention to these topics.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>It’s important to provide support to people who <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults">suffer from loneliness</a>, just as it is important to pay attention to the policy challenges that come from large societal changes such as the <a href="http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone">rise of living alone</a>. However, inaccurate, over-simplified narratives are unhelpful to really understand these complex challenges.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>There is an epidemic of headlines that claim we are experiencing a “loneliness epidemic”, but there is no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading --> <h2>Social media and social networks</h2> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>The rise of social media platforms</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":3} --> <h3>Are Facebook and other social media platforms bad for our well-being?</h3> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp-block-tombstone 30061 --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Facebook, Youtube, Whatsapp, WeChat, and Instagram are the top five social media platforms globally, with over <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/users-by-social-media-platform">one billion active users each</a>. In most rich countries the proportion of young people using online social networks <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/percentage-of-young-people-engaging-in-social-networking-online">exceeds 90%</a> and teens spend on average <a href="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/daily-time-spent-on-the-internet-by-young-people">more than 4 hours online every day</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>We’re repeatedly told in the news that social media is bad for us. The stories are often alarming, suggesting social media and smartphones are responsible for sweeping negative trends, <a href="https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression">from rising suicide rates in the US</a>, to <a href="https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-families/social-media-mental-health-negative-effects-depression-anxiety-addiction-memory-a8307196.html">widespread loss in memory, and reduced sleep and attention spans</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>These worrying headlines often go together with implicit or explicit recommendations to limit the amount of time we spend on social media. Indeed, smartphones today come with built-in “screen time” apps that let us track and limit how much time we spend online.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>At the same time, most of us would agree that digital social media platforms can make our lives easier in many ways – opening doors to new information, connecting us with people who are far away, and helping us to be more flexible with work.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>What does the research tell us about the causal impact of social media use on our well-being?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In a nutshell: From my reading of the scientific literature, I do not believe that the available evidence today supports the sweeping newspaper headlines.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Yes, there is evidence suggesting a causal negative effect, but the size of these causal effects is heterogeneous and much, much smaller than the news headlines suggest.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>There are still plenty of good reasons to reflect on the impact of social media in society, and there is much we can all learn to make better use of these complex digital platforms. But this requires going beyond universal claims.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Let’s take a look at the evidence.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Comparisons across individuals</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Most of the news stories that claim social media has a negative impact on well-being rely on data from surveys comparing individuals with different levels of social media use as evidence. In the chart, I show one concrete example of this type of correlational analysis.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart plots the average amount of time that people spend on social media each day, among people who are and aren’t happy with the amount of time spent on these platforms.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The data comes from an app called <a href="https://inthemoment.io/">Moment</a>, which tracks the amount of time users spend on social media platforms on their smartphones. The app also asks people a yes/no question: “Are you happy with your time spent?”</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>As we can see there is quite a bit of heterogeneity across platforms, but the pattern is clear: People who say they are happy with how much time they spend on social media spend less time on these platforms. Or put differently, using social media more heavily is correlated with less satisfaction.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This is certainly interesting, but we should be careful not to jump to conclusions – the correlation actually raises as many questions as it answers.{ref}A concrete example of how this correlational evidence is reported in the news can be found in <a href="https://www.economist.com/graphic-detail/2018/05/18/how-heavy-use-of-social-media-is-linked-to-mental-illness">this article from The Economist</a>, where a very similar chart is presented under the headline “How heavy use of social media is linked to mental illness”.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:image {"id":24578} --> <figure class="wp-block-image"><img src="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/09/TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-–-happy-vs-unhappy-users-608x550.png" alt="" class="wp-image-24578"/></figure> <!-- /wp:image --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Does this pattern hold if we control for user characteristics like age and gender? Would we get similar results if we focused on other measures of well-being beyond ‘happy with time spent’?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The answer to both questions is ‘no’. Depending on what outcome variables you focus on, and depending on which demographic characteristics you account for, you will get a different result. It is therefore not surprising that some empirical academic studies have found negative correlations; while others actually report positive correlations.{ref}Here are some examples of studies reporting a positive correlation between social media use and subjective well-being: - Kim, J. and Lee, J. (2011). The facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number of face- book friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. CyberPsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(6):359–364 - Gonzales, A. L. and Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my facebook wall: Effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(1-2):79–83. - Valenzuela, S., Park, N., and Kee, K. F. (2009). Is there social capital in a social network site?: Facebook use and college students’ life satisfaction, trust, and participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14(4):875–901.{/ref} </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski <a href="https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-018-0506-1">published a paper</a> earlier this year in the journal <em>Nature</em> where they illustrated that given the flexibility to analyze the data (i.e. given the number of possible choices researchers have when it comes to processing and interpreting the vast data from these large surveys), scientists <a href="https://socialsciences.nature.com/users/200472-amy-orben/posts/42763-beyond-cherry-picking">could have written thousands of papers</a> describing positive, negative and non-significant associations. Different ways of measuring well-being and social media use will yield different results, even for the same population.{ref} It’s also important to mention that the correlations between social media use and wellbeing are not generally comparable across countries and time. For example, commentators<a href="https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression"> often highlight</a> that depression and suicide rates among adolescents in the US have been going up ever since the rise of smartphone, and that this is a clear red flag. Yet as Andrew Przybylski explained <a href="http://rationallyspeakingpodcast.org/show/rs-237-andy-przybylski-on-is-screen-time-bad-for-you.html">in a recent interview</a>, this is just not a general observation: <em>“You don't see things like it in more tech-saturated countries, or in other industrialized countries. You don't see, two or three years ahead of the United States, the South Koreans and the Japanese having spiking rates of self-harm or depression”</em>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Even the answers to some of the most fundamental questions are unclear: Do we actually know in which direction the relationship might be going? Does frequent social media use translate into lower happiness, or is it the other way around – are anxious, stressed or depressed people particularly prone to use social media?</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This takes us to another branch of the literature: longitudinal studies that track individuals over time to measure changes in social media use and well-being.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Studies of social media use and well-being over time</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>One longitudinal study that has received much attention on this subject was <a href="https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143">published</a> by Holly Shakya and Nicholas Christakis in the <em>American Journal of Epidemiology</em> in 2017. It used data from a survey that tracked a group of 5,208 Americans over the period 2013 - 2015, and found an increase in Facebook activity was associated with a future <em>decrease</em> in reported mental health.{ref}The full reference is: Shakya, H. B., & Christakis, N. A. (2017). Association of Facebook use with compromised well-being: A longitudinal study. <em>American journal of epidemiology</em>, <em>185</em>(3), 203-211. Online <a href="https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Two years later, Amy Orben, Tobias Dienlin and Andrew Przybylski <a href="https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf">published a paper</a> in the <em>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences</em> using a similar source of data. They relied on a longitudinal survey from the UK covering 12,672 teenagers over the period 2009 - 2016, and reached a different conclusion. They found that there was a small and reciprocal relationship: social media use predicted small decreases in life satisfaction; but it was also the case that decreasing life satisfaction predicted subsequent increases in social media use.{ref}The full reference is: Orben, A., Dienlin, T., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(21), 10226-10228. Online <a href="https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Summarizing their research in <em>The Guardian</em>, Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski <a href="https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2019/jul/07/too-much-screen-time-hurts-kids-where-is-evidence">explained</a>: <em>“we did find some small trends over time – these were mostly clustered in data provided by teenage girls… But – and this is key – it’s not an exaggeration to say that these effects were minuscule by the standards of science and trivial if you want to inform personal parenting decisions. Our results indicated that 99.6% of the variability in adolescent girls’ satisfaction with life had nothing to do with how much they used social media.”</em></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In their paper Orben and co-authors argue again that these large datasets allow many different types of empirical tests; so it is <a href="https://slatestarcodex.com/2014/12/12/beware-the-man-of-one-study/">natural to expect conflicting results across studies</a>, particularly if there is noise in measurement and the true effect sizes are small.{ref}What’s more, if the cutoff for a statistically significant finding is 5% and you test 100 independent hypotheses of which in reality none are true, you will (in expectation) still find 5 significant associations in either direction simply by chance. Here is a good example and discussion of how this works: <a href="https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1">https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p> Orben and co-authors tested thousands of empirical tests and indeed, some of these tests could have been interpreted on their own as evidence of a strong negative effect for social media – but clearly the broader picture is important. When looking at the results from all their thousands of tests, they concluded that social media effects were nuanced, small at best and reciprocal over time.{ref}One more piece of evidence that confirms this conclusion, comes from a more recent long-run study that tracked 500 adolescents for eight consecutive years, between the ages of 13 and 20, and found that <em> "increased time spent on social media was not associated with increased mental health issues across development."</em> The full reference is Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L., & Booth, M. (2019). Does time spent using social media impact mental health?: An eight year longitudinal study. <em>Computers in Human Behavior</em>, 106160. Available online <a href="https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0747563219303723">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Facebook experiments</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Establishing causal impacts through observational studies that track the well-being of individuals over time is difficult. </p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>First, there are measurement issues. Long-run surveys that track people are expensive and impose a high burden on participants, so they do not allow in-depth high-frequency data collection, and instead focus on broad trends across a wide range of topics. Orben and co-authors, for example, rely on the <a href="https://www.understandingsociety.ac.uk/"><em>Understanding Society Survey</em></a> from the UK, which covers a wide range of themes such as family life, education, employment, finance, health and wellbeing. Specifically on social media use, this survey only ask how many hours teenagers remember using apps during normal weekdays, which is of course an informative but noisy measure of actual use (a fact that Orben and co-authors mention in their paper).</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Second, there are limitations from unobservable variables. Frequent users of social media are likely different from less frequent users in ways that are hard to measure – no matter how many questions you include in a survey, there will always be relevant factors you cannot account for in the analysis.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Given these limitations, an obvious alternative is to run an experiment: you can, for example, offer people money to stop using Facebook for a while and then check the effect by comparing these “treated participants” against a control group that is allowed to continue using Facebook as usual.{ref} Experiments like this will tell us the short-run effect among experiment participants. This is of course not a silver bullet – one could raise concerns about how generalizable the estimates are (e.g. Are participants representative of the general population? Do effects remain over time?). No method is perfect, but experiments do address the issue of causality, so they provide critical evidence to understand the link between social media and well-being.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Several recent papers followed this approach. Here I’ll discuss <a href="https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf">one of them in particular</a>, because I find its approach particularly compelling. The analysis relies on a much larger sample than other experiments, and the researchers registered a <a href="https://www.socialscienceregistry.org/trials/3409">pre-analysis plan</a> to insure themselves against the ‘analytical flexibility’ criticisms discussed above.{ref} The full reference is: Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2019). The welfare effects of social media (No. w25514). National Bureau of Economic Research. Available online <a href="https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf">here</a>.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>This experiment was done by four economists: Hunt Allcott, Sarah Eichmeyer, Luca Braghieri and Matthew Gentzkow. They recruited 2753 Facebook users in the US, and randomly selected half of them to stop using Facebook for four weeks. They found that deactivating Facebook led to small but statistically significant improvements in some measures of self-reported well-being.{ref} They recruit participants by giving them electronic gift cards. Loosely speaking, the idea was that participants were paid to play a lottery: Everybody received the monetary incentive, but only those who ‘lost the lottery’ had to deactivate their account.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The chart shows a summary of their estimated effect sizes. As we can see, for all measures the effects are small (amounting to only around a tenth of the standard deviation of the studied variable), and in most cases the effects are actually not statistically significant (the ‘whiskers’ denoting 95% confidence intervals often include an effect of size zero).{ref} The paper provides the following definition for the variables: <em>The happiness variable is the average response to two questions from the Subjective Happiness Scale (Lyubomirsky and Lepper 1999), asking how happy participants were over the past four weeks and how happy they were compared to their peers. Life satisfaction is the sum of responses to three questions from the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al. 1985), such as the level of agreement with the statement, “During the past 4 weeks, I was satisfied with my life.” Loneliness is the Three-Item Loneliness Scale (Hughes et al. 2004). Finally, depressed, anxious, absorbed, and bored reflect how much of the time during the past four weeks respondents felt each emotion, using questions from the European Social Survey well-being module (Huppert et al. 2009). The daily text messages allowed us to measure the aspects of subjective well-being that are most important to record in the moment instead of retrospectively. This approach builds on the Experience Sampling Method of Csikszentmihalyi and Larson (2014) and Stone and Shiffman (1994). The variable SMS happiness is the answer to the question, “Overall, how happy do you feel right now on a scale from 1 (not at all happy) to 10 (completely happy)?” The variable SMS positive emotion is an indicator variable for whether the participant reports a positive emotion when asked, “What best describes how you felt over the last ten minutes?”, with possible responses such as “angry,” “worried,” “loving/tender,” etc. Finally, SMS not lonely uses the answer to the question, “How lonely are you feeling right now on a scale from 1 (not at all lonely) to 10 (very lonely)?”</em>{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Allcott and co-authors also compare the treatment effects against the observational correlations in their sample and conclude: “<em>the magnitudes of our causal effects are far smaller than those we would have estimated using the correlational approach of much prior literature”</em>.{ref} To get a sense of the magnitudes of these effects, the authors discuss effect sizes in their original units, focusing on the measures with the largest effects. Happiness is the average response to two questions on a scale from 1 (not a very happy person) to 7 (a very happy person). The control group endline average is 4.47 out of a possible 7, and deactivation caused an average increase of 0.12. This is of course very, very small. In fact, the authors explain that given how small these effect sizes are, they would have been unlikely to have sufficient statistical power to detect any effects if they had used sample sizes similar to those from previous experiments in this field, all of which are much smaller.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The relatively small experimental effect of social media use on subjective well-being has been replicated. <a href="https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462">Another experiment</a> conducted almost at the same time and with a very similar approach, produced similar results.{ref} In <a href="https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462">this other experiment</a>, quitting Facebook for a week led to a small but statistically significant decrease in self-reported feelings of depression, yet there was no significant effect on other measures of life satisfaction. The full reference is: Mosquera, R., Odunowo, M. M., McNamara, T., Guo, X., & Petrie, R. (2018). The Economic Effects of Facebook. {/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:image {"id":55044,"sizeSlug":"large","linkDestination":"none"} --> <figure class="wp-block-image size-large"><img src="https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2022/12/subjective-well-being-1-v2-full-530x550.png" alt="" class="wp-image-55044"/></figure> <!-- /wp:image --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Mechanisms</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In the US, where many of these studies have been conducted, roughly two-thirds of people <a href="https://www.journalism.org/2018/09/10/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-2018/">get news from social media</a>, and these platforms have already become a <a href="https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/12/10/social-media-outpaces-print-newspapers-in-the-u-s-as-a-news-source/">more widely accessed source of news than print newspapers</a>.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>I think this link between social media, news consumption and well-being is key.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>In their experiment, Allcott and coauthors found that quitting Facebook did not lead people to use alternative online or offline news sources; so those in the treatment group reported spending less time consuming news overall. This tells us that the effect of social media on well-being is not only relatively small, but also likely mediated by the specific types of content and information that people are exposed to.{ref}This effect has also been replicated: The other contemporary experiment that followed a similar approach (<a href="https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462">Mosquera et al. 2019</a>), found that individuals facing a Facebook restriction reduced their news consumption.{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The fact that news consumption via social media might be an important factor affecting well-being is not surprising if we consider that news are typically biased towards negative content, and there is <a href="https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract">empirical research</a> suggesting people are triggered, at a physiological level, when exposed to negative news content.{ref} A new paper published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences concludes that all around the world, the average human is more physiologically activated by negative than by positive news stories. The authors come to this conclusion through an experiment: Participants watched 7 randomly ordered BBC World News stories on a laptop computer while wearing noise-cancelling headphones and sensors on their fingers to capture skin conductance and blood volume pulse. The experiment was conducted with 1,156 participants across 17 countries: Brazil, Canada, Chile, China, Denmark, France, Ghana, India, Israel, Italy, Japan, New Zealand, Russia, Senegal, Sweden, the United Kingdom, and the United States. The full reference is Soroka, S., Fournier P., & Nir L., (2019) Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1908369116. Available online <a href="https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract">here</a>.<br>{/ref}</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Building and reinforcing a scary overarching narrative around “the terrible negative effects of social media on well-being” is unhelpful because this fails to recognise that social media is a large and evolving ecosystem where billions of people interact and consume information in many different ways.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>What are the key takeaways?</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The first takeaway is that the association between social media and well-being is complex and reciprocal, which means that simple correlations can be misleading. A careful analysis of survey data reveals that, yes, there is a correlation between social media and well-being; but the relationship works both ways. This becomes clear from the longitudinal studies: Higher use of social media predicts decreases in life satisfaction; and decreasing life satisfaction also predicts subsequent increases in social media use.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>The second takeaway is that the causal effect of social media on well-being is likely small for the average person. The best empirical evidence suggests the impact is much smaller than many news stories suggest and most people believe.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>There is much to be learned about how to make better use of these digital platforms, and there is an important discussion to be had about the opportunity costs of spending a large fraction of our time online. But for this we need to look beyond the sweeping newspaper headlines.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>We need research with more granular data to unpack diverse use patterns, to understand the different effects that certain types of content have on specific population groups. Time alone is a poor metric to gauge effects. As Andrew Przybylski put it: nobody would argue we should study the causes of obesity by investigating ‘food time’.</p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p>Going forward, the conversation in policy and the news should be much more about strategies to promote positive content and interactions, than about one-size-fits-all restrictions on social media ‘screen time’.<br></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> <!-- wp:heading --> <h2>Data Sources</h2> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28.</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Selected towns and cities</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093">https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093</a></li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4> Cigna - Loneliness Report</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Survey of 20,000 Americans</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Self-reported loneliness</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> US</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href="https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america">https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america</a> </li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>Office for National Statistics - Loneliness Report</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Community Life Survey</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Self-reported loneliness</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> UK</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href="https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10">https://www.ons.gov.uk</a></li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>World Value Survey</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Representative national surveys</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Share of people who interact with friends at least once per month</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Selected countries</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href="http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/">http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/</a></li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>United Nations Database on Household Size and Composition 2018</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Percentage of one-person households as a ratio of total households</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Global, by country</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href="https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840">https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840</a></li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>OECD – How's Life? 2017 Measuring Well-being</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Percentage of people who report that they have friends or relatives whom they can count on in times of trouble</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Global, by country</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href="https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en">https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en</a></li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:heading {"level":4} --> <h4>OECD – Society at a Glance</h4> <!-- /wp:heading --> <!-- wp:list --> <ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Social media use</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Global, by country</li><li><strong>Link:</strong> <a href="https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en">https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en</a></li></ul> <!-- /wp:list --> <!-- wp:paragraph --> <p></p> <!-- /wp:paragraph --> | { "id": "wp-29766", "slug": "social-connections-and-loneliness", "content": { "toc": [], "body": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This article was first published in February 2020.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Available research shows that social connections are important for our well-being. Having support from family and friends is important for our happiness and health, and is also instrumental to our ability to share information, learn from others, and seize economic opportunities.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections across countries and over time, and review available evidence on how and why social connections and loneliness affect our health and emotional welfare, as well as our material well-being.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Despite the fact that there is a clear link between social connections and well-being, more research is needed to understand causal mechanisms, effect sizes and changes over time. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "As we show here, oversimplified narratives that compare loneliness with smoking, or that claim we are living a 'loneliness epidemic', are wrong and unhelpful. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "entry-summary", "items": [ { "slug": "how-important-are-social-connections-for-our-health", "text": "Social connections matter directly for our happiness and health." }, { "slug": "the-importance-of-personal-relations-for-economic-outcomes", "text": "Social connections are instrumental to prosperity and material well-being." }, { "slug": "causality-and-implications", "text": "Despite the clear link between social connections and well-being, more research is needed to understand causal mechanisms and effect sizes. In particular, more research is needed to pin down the effect that loneliness has on life expectancy." }, { "slug": "loneliness-solitude-and-social-isolation", "text": "Solitary living arrangements are becoming increasingly common around the world, but there is no evidence that self-reported loneliness is increasing over time. This can be explained by the fact that loneliness and aloneness are not the same." }, { "slug": "the-rise-of-social-media-platforms", "text": "Social media platforms have transformed social connections around the world. " }, { "slug": "are-facebook-and-other-social-media-platforms-bad-for-our-well-being", "text": "The causal effect that social media has on adolescent well-being is likely negative, but it is also likely heterogeneous across the population, and much smaller than newspaper headlines suggest." } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Related articles", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/trust", "children": [ { "text": "Interpersonal Trust", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " \u2013 Trust is an essential part of social connections. Trust is crucial for community, wellbeing, and effective cooperation.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/internet", "children": [ { "text": "Internet", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " \u2013 For many, the internet is now essential for work, finding information, and connecting with others.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta", "children": [ { "text": "Global Health", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " \u2013 Social connections matter for health. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in global health.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction", "children": [ { "text": "Happiness and Life Satisfaction", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " \u2013 Social connections matter for our happiness. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in happiness and life satisfaction across societies and locations.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "url": "#all-charts", "children": [ { "text": "See all interactive charts on loneliness and social connections \u2193", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Social connections", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 1, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "How important are social connections for our health?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Dr. Vivek Murthy, former ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Surgeon_General_of_the_United_States", "children": [ { "text": "Surgeon General", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " of the United States, recently ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.vivekmurthy.com/single-post/2017/10/10/Work-and-the-Loneliness-Epidemic-Harvard-Business-Review", "children": [ { "text": "wrote", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ": \u201cLoneliness and weak social connections are associated with a reduction in lifespan similar to that caused by smoking 15 cigarettes a day\u201d.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This \u201815 cigarettes a day\u2019 figure has been reproduced and reported in the news many times, under headlines such as \u201cLoneliness is as lethal as smoking 15 cigarettes per day\u201d.{ref}There are dozens of articles reporting the \u201815 cigarettes a day\u2019 figure. See for example ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.webmd.com/balance/news/20180504/loneliness-rivals-obesity-smoking-as-health-risk", "children": [ { "text": "this in WebMD", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", and ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-5181559/Loneliness-bad-health-smoking-15-day.html", "children": [ { "text": "this in The Daily Mail", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". From what I gather, all the articles that cite this figure trace back to Dr. Vivek Murthy and a meta-analysis by Holt-Lunstad et al. (2015), which I discuss in more detail below in this post. When I reviewed this meta-analysis I did find the claim that loneliness is a risk factor comparable to smoking; but I could not find the calculations that lead to the 15 cigarettes benchmark. It\u2019s still unclear to me what the unit of loneliness is and what this translates into in terms of years of lives lost. However, given that 15 cigarettes per day is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/consumption-per-smoker-per-day-bounds", "children": [ { "text": "about average consumption among smokers", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", it\u2019s possible that the benchmark comes from comparing excess mortality of smoking for the average smoker, vs. excess mortality of loneliness for average levels of loneliness.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "It is indeed quite a shocking comparison since around ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/smoking-and-secondhand-deaths", "children": [ { "text": "7 million deaths", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " globally are attributed to smoking every year, and back-of-the-envelope calculations published in medical journals say ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.bmj.com/content/bmj/320/7226/53.1.full.pdf?casa_token=Fcl9ywjt31gAAAAA:9FnQFmC50vZclWEndIVf8Vf4uwlEsKIiQeeQtFXm11_06jJwLcgZn1MS4LPyFsFVnrcCsSSHXnv5", "children": [ { "text": "one cigarette reduces your lifespan by 11 minutes", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Here we dig deeper to try to understand what the data and research tell us about the link between social relations and health. In a nutshell, my reading of the evidence is as follows:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "numbered-list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There is a huge amount of evidence showing individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in their life.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There is credible theory and explanation of biological mechanisms, whereby isolation can set off unconscious surveillance for social threat, producing cognitive biases, reducing sleep and affecting hormones.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "It's very likely there is a causal link, but there is no credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect that loneliness has on key metrics of health, such as life expectancy.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect of loneliness on health doesn't mean we should dismiss the available evidence. But it does show that more research is needed.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Observational studies: A first look at the data", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Measuring loneliness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 4, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Psychologists and social neuroscientists often refer to loneliness as ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2008-07755-000", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "painful", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " isolation", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". The emphasis on painful is there to make a clear distinction between solitude \u2013 the state of being alone \u2013 and subjective loneliness, which is the distressing feeling that comes from unmet expectations of the types of interpersonal relationships we wish to have.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Researchers use several kinds of data to measure solitude and loneliness. The most common source of data are surveys where people are asked about different aspects of their lives, including whether they live alone, how much time they spend with other people in a given window of time (e.g. \u2018last week\u2019) or specific context (e.g. \u2018at social events, clubs or places of worship\u2019); and whether they experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. \u2018I have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with\u2019). Researchers sometimes study these survey responses separately, but often they also aggregate them in a composite index.{ref}The most popular composite index is the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/UCLA_Loneliness_Scale", "children": [ { "text": "UCLA Loneliness Scale", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", which was first proposed in 1978, and is based on a 20-item scale designed to measure subjective feelings of loneliness. You can read more about it ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://fetzer.org/sites/default/files/images/stories/pdf/selfmeasures/Self_Measures_for_Loneliness_and_Interpersonal_Problems_UCLA_LONELINESS.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Surveys ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0898264312460275", "children": [ { "text": "confirm", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that people respond differently to questions about subjective loneliness and physical social isolation, which suggests people do understand these as two distinct issues.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the chart here I've put together estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness from various sources. The fact that we see such high levels of loneliness, with substantial divergence across countries, explains why this is an important and active research area. Indeed, there are literally hundreds of papers that have used survey data to explore the link between loneliness, solitude, and health. Below is an overview of what these studies find.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The link between loneliness and physical health", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 4, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Most papers studying the link between loneliness and health find that both objective solitude (e.g. living alone) and subjective loneliness (e.g. frequent self-reported feelings of loneliness) are correlated with higher morbidity (i.e. illness) and higher mortality (i.e. likelihood of death).", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The relationship between health and loneliness can of course go both ways: lonely people may see their health deteriorate with time; but it may also be the case that people who suffer from poor health end up feeling more lonely later down the line.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Because of this two-way relationship it\u2019s important to go beyond cross-sectional correlations and focus on longitudinal studies \u2013 these are studies where researchers track the same individuals over time to see if loneliness predicts illness or mortality in the future, after controlling for baseline behaviors and health status.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The evidence from longitudinal studies shows that people who experience loneliness during a period of their life tend to be more likely to have worse health later down the line. In the Netherlands, for example, ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://academic.oup.com/aje/article-pdf/146/6/510/455063/146-6-510.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "researchers found", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that self-reported loneliness among adults aged 55-85 predicted mortality several months later, and this was true after controlling for age, sex, chronic diseases, alcohol use, smoking, self-assessed health condition, and functional limitations.{ref}Penninx, B. W., Van Tilburg, T., Kriegsman, D. M., Deeg, D. J., Boeke, A. J. P., & van Eijk, J. T. M. (1997). Effects of social support and personal coping resources on mortality in older age: The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. American journal of epidemiology, 146(6), 510-519.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Most studies focus either on subjective loneliness, or on objective isolation. But some studies try to compare both. In a recent meta-analysis covering 70 longitudinal studies, the authors ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691614568352", "children": [ { "text": "write", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ":", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": " \u201cWe found no differences between measures of objective and subjective social isolation. Results remain consistent across gender, length of follow-up, and world region.\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " And in the concluding section they highlight that, in their interpretation of the evidence, ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cthe risk associated with social isolation and loneliness is comparable with well-established risk factors for mortality\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "; which include smoking and obesity.{ref}Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on psychological science, 10(2), 227-237.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The link between mental health and subjective well-being", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 4, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In another much-cited review of the evidence, Louise Hawkley and John Cacioppo, two leading experts on this topic, ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3874845/", "children": [ { "text": "concluded", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cperhaps the most striking finding in this literature is the breadth of emotional and cognitive processes and outcomes that seem susceptible to the influence of loneliness\u201d.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "{ref}Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of behavioral medicine, 40(2), 218-227. {/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Researchers have found that loneliness correlates with subsequent increases in symptoms related to ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/dementia-death-rates", "children": [ { "text": "dementia", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health#depression", "children": [ { "text": "depression", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", and many other issues related to ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health", "children": [ { "text": "mental health", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": "; and this holds after controlling for demographic variables, objective social isolation, stress, and baseline levels of cognitive function.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There is also research that suggests a link between loneliness and lower ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction", "children": [ { "text": "happiness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", and we discuss this in more detail ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-friends", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Mechanisms", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Experiments with social animals, like rats, ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/106/52/22393.full.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "show", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that induced isolation can lead to a higher risk of death from cancer. Humans and rats are of course very different; but experts, such as Hawkley and Cacioppo, argue that these experiments are important because they tell us something meaningful about a shared biological mechanism. In a review of the evidence, Susan Pinker writes: \u201cIf our big brains evolved to interact, loneliness would be an early warning system\u2014a built-in alarm that sent a biological signal to members who had somehow become separated from the group\u201d.{ref}Pinker, S. (2015). The village effect: How face-to-face contact can make us healthier and happier. Vintage Canada.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Indeed, there\u2019s evidence of social regulation of gene expression in humans: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/108/7/3080.full.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "studies suggest", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " perceived loneliness can switch on/off genes that regulate our immune systems, and it is this what then affects the health of humans, or other animals that evolved with similar defence mechanisms.{ref}Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). Transcript origin analysis identifies antigen-presenting cells as primary targets of socially regulated gene expression in leukocytes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 3080-3085.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Causality and implications", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The bulk of evidence from observational studies and biological mechanisms, described above, implies that loneliness most likely matters for our health and well being. But do we really know how much it matters relative to other important risk factors?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The key point here is that estimates are likely biased to some extent.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The findings from longitudinal studies that track individuals over time are insightful, but they cannot rule out that the relationship might be partly driven by other factors that we cannot observe. Even the studies linking loneliness and genetics can be subject to this ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://mru.org/dictionary-economics/omitted-variable-economics", "children": [ { "text": "omitted-variable bias", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", because a genetic predisposition to loneliness may drive both loneliness and health outcomes.{ref}Two concrete papers that show this is a real concern are:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "- Abdellaoui, A., Sanchez-Roige, S., Sealock, J., Treur, J. L., Dennis, J., Fontanillas, P., \u2026 & Baselmans, B. (2018). Phenome-wide investigation of health outcomes associated with genetic predisposition to loneliness. bioRxiv, 468835.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "-Abdellaoui, A., Chen, H. Y., Willemsen, G., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Verweij, K. J., \u2026 & Cacioppo, J. T. (2019). Associations between loneliness and personality are mostly driven by a genetic association with neuroticism. Journal of personality, 87(2), 386-397.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "I could not find credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect.{ref}If you know of relevant studies that do provide such estimates, please let me know at [email protected].{/ref} But the fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect doesn't mean we should dismiss the available evidence. On the contrary \u2013 it would be great if we had evidence from randomized control trials that test positive interventions to reduce loneliness, to understand better if the \u201815 cigarettes per day\u2019 comparison from the Surgeon General of the US is roughly correct, at least for the average person.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Having a better understanding of the magnitude of the effect is important, not only because loneliness is common, but also because it\u2019s complex and unequally experienced by people around the world.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "As the chart above shows, there are large differences in self-reported loneliness across countries. We should understand how important these differences are for the distribution of health and well-being.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Are we happier when we spend more time with others?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In 1938, a group of Harvard researchers decided to start a research program to track the lives of a group of young men, in what eventually became one of the longest and most famous longitudinal studies of its kind. The idea was to track the development of a group of teenage boys through periodic interviews and medical checkups, with the aim of understanding how their health and well-being evolved as they grew up.{ref}For historical context: The Harvard Graduate School of Education \u2013 the first to admit women \u2013 opened admissions to women in 1920; and the Harvard Medical School accepted its first female enrollees in 1945, even though the first woman applied in 1847. You can read more about the hard-earned gains for women at Harvard here: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/", "children": [ { "text": "https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": "{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Today, more than 80 years later, it is one of the longest running research programmes in social science. It is called the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.adultdevelopmentstudy.org/", "children": [ { "text": "Harvard Study of Adult Development", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " and it is still running. The program started with 724 boys, and researchers continue to monitor today the health and well-being of those initial participants who are still alive, most in their late 90s.{ref}The original participants for this study came from two groups: a cohort of 268 men, 19 years of age, from the Harvard classes of 1939-1944; and a group of 456 caucasian men, ages 11-16, from underprivileged neighborhoods of Boston. Over the years the project evolved, enrolling more participants, and many of the children of the original participants are included in the study today.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This is a unique scientific exercise \u2013 most longitudinal studies do not last this long because too many participants drop out, researchers move on to other projects (or even die), or the funding dries up.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "So what have we learned from this unique study?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Robert Waldinger, the current director of the study, summarized \u2013 in what is now one of the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.ted.com/talks/robert_waldinger_what_makes_a_good_life_lessons_from_the_longest_study_on_happiness?language=en", "children": [ { "text": "most viewed TED Talks to date", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " \u2013 the findings from decades of research. The main result, he concluded, is that social connections are one of the most important factors for people\u2019s ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction", "children": [ { "text": "happiness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " and ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta", "children": [ { "text": "health", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". He said: \u201cThose who kept warm relationships got to live longer and happier, and the loners often died earlier.\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Here I take a closer look at the evidence and show more research that finds a consistent link between social connections and happiness. But before we get to the details, let me explain why I think this link is important.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "As most people can attest from personal experience, striving for happiness is not easy. In fact, the search for happiness can become a source of ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "unhappiness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " \u2013 there are studies that show actively pursuing happiness ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "can end up decreasing it", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The data shows that ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#the-link-between-happiness-and-income", "children": [ { "text": "income and happiness are clearly related", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": "; but we also know from surveys that people often ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/spc3.12386", "children": [ { "text": "overestimate the impact of income on happiness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". Social relations might be the missing link: In rich countries, where minimum material living conditions are ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/extreme-poverty#historical-poverty-in-today-s-rich-countries", "children": [ { "text": "often satisfied", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", people may struggle to become happier because they are targeting material rather than social goals.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The cross-sectional correlation between happiness and friends", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/wvs.jsp", "children": [ { "text": "World Values Survey (WVS)", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " is a large cross-country research project that collects data from a series of representative national surveys. In the fourth wave (1999-2004) the WVS asked respondents hundreds of questions about their lives, including whether they were part of social or religious groups, how often they spent time with friends, and whether they felt happy with their life.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "By comparing self-reported happiness among those with and without frequent social interactions, we can get an idea of whether there is indeed a raw correlation between happiness and social relations across different societies.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the chart I show this comparison: The green points correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends at least once per month; while the blue dots correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends less often.{ref}The question on frequency of interaction with friends was asked in the context of time spent at work, clubs or places of worship. The options included \u2018weekly\u2019, \u2018once or twice a month\u2019, \u2018only a few times a month\u2019, and \u2018not at all\u2019. The options for happiness, on the other hand, included \u2018very happy\u2019, \u2018quite happy\u2019, \u2018not very happy\u2019 and \u2018not at all happy\u2019. In the chart I took this data, and I split the sample in two: I classified respondents as \u2018often meeting with friends\u2019 if they said they met with friends once a month or more frequently; and I classified them as \u2018rarely meeting with friends\u2019 if they said they met with friends \u2018once a year\u2019 or \u2018not at all\u2019. Then, for each of these two groups, I plotted the share who reported being \u2018very happy\u2019 or 'quite happy\u2019.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This chart shows that in almost all countries, people who often spend time with their friends report to be happier than those who spend less time with friends.{ref}The estimates have of course a margin of error that varies with the survey size. The difference between the green and blue dots is statistically significant for about half of the countries (i.e. for about half of the countries in this chart, the 95% confidence intervals around the blue and green dots would not overlap). {/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "alt": "", "size": "wide", "type": "image", "filename": "happiness-and-friends-v3-e1564090552891.png", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The link between social relations and happiness over time", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart above gives a cross-sectional perspective \u2013 it\u2019s just a snapshot that compares different people at a given point in time. What happens if we look at changes in social relations and happiness over time?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There is a large academic literature in medicine and psychology that shows individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in life (you can read more about this in my ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being", "children": [ { "text": "post on social relations and health", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": "); and similarly, there are also many studies that show that changes in social relations predict changes in happiness and life satisfaction.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "One of the research papers that draws on data from the famous Harvard Study of Adult Development, for example, looked at the experiences of 82 married participants and their spouses, and found that greater self-reported couple attachment predicted lower levels of depression and greater life satisfaction 2.5 years later.{ref}Waldinger, R. J., Cohen, S., Schulz, M. S., & Crowell, J. A. (2015). Security of attachment to spouses in late life: Concurrent and prospective links with cognitive and emotional well-being. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 516-529. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4579537/", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": " Other studies with larger population samples have also found a similar cross-temporal link: perceived social isolation predicts subsequent changes in depressive symptoms but not vice versa, and this holds after controlling for demographic variables and stress.{ref}See for example:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "- Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and aging, 25(2), 453.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "- Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and aging, 21(1), 140.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Searching for happiness is typically an intentional and active pursuit. Is it the case that people tend to become happier when they purposely decide to improve their social relations?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This is a tough empirical question to test; but ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0956797618761660", "children": [ { "text": "a recent study", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " found evidence pointing in this direction.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Using a large representative survey in Germany, researchers asked participants to report, in text format, ideas for how they could improve their life satisfaction. Based on these answers, the researchers then investigated which types of ideas predicted changes in life satisfaction one year later.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The researchers found that those who reported socially-engaged strategies (e.g. \u201cI plan to spend more time with friends and family\u201d) often reported improvements in life satisfaction one year later; while those who described other non-social active pursuits (e.g. \u201cI plan to find a better job\u201d) did not report increased life satisfaction.{ref}The researchers used an open question: \u201cWhat could you do to ensure that you will be more satisfied in the future?\u201d. There are of course many types of answers that people can provide to such an open-ended question, so the authors decided to classify each individual answer into one of two categories, depending on whether or not the respondents described a \u2018socially engaged strategy\u2019. This process of classifying answers could naturally raise questions: the authors could manipulate the subjective classification of answers to fit their favourite hypothesis. In order to insure themselves against this criticism, the researchers relied on three independent coders, and they stipulated the classification method and the expected results in a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://blogs.worldbank.org/impactevaluations/a-pre-analysis-plan-checklist", "children": [ { "text": "pre-analysis plan", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". You find more details in the paper: Rohrer, J. M., Richter, D., Br\u00fcmmer, M., Wagner, G. G., & Schmukle, S. C. (2018). Successfully striving for happiness: Socially engaged pursuits predict increases in life satisfaction. Psychological science, 29(8), 1291-1298.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Are we happier when we spend more time with others?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "From decades of research we know that social relations predict mental well-being over time; and from a recent study we also know that people who actively decide to improve their social relations often report becoming happier. So yes, people are happier when they spend more time with friends.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Does this mean that if we have an exogenous shock to our social relations this will have a permanent negative effect on our happiness?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "We can\u2019t really answer this with the available evidence. More research is needed to really understand the causal mechanisms that drive the link between happiness and social relations.{ref}The key point to keep in mind is that we are not talking about experimental evidence. People who frequently interact with friends, or people who say they want to engage more in \u2018social strategies to improve their lives\u2019 are likely different; and although the researchers do try to control for this, it is very hard to measure all the relevant characteristics that might help drive life satisfaction. Similarly, asking people about strategies to improve their lives is difficult, and although the qualitative strategy from the paper above is interesting, it\u2019s still hard to draw accurate comparisons: it\u2019s possible that \u2018non-social\u2019 strategies, for example, are more difficult or stressful to pursue; so part of the lower apparent impact that these strategies have on happiness might come from the fact that some people are trying but failing to find a better job.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Despite this, however, I think that we should take the available observational evidence seriously. In a way, the causal impact from a random shock is less interesting than the evidence from active strategies that people might take to improve their happiness. People who get divorced, for example, often report a short-term drop in life satisfaction; but over time they tend to recover and eventually end up being more satisfied with their life than shortly before they divorced (you can read more about this in our ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#life-satisfaction-through-life-events", "children": [ { "text": "entry on happiness and life satisfaction", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ").", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "It makes sense to consider the possibility that healthy social relationships are a key missing piece for human wellbeing. Among other things, this would help explain the paradoxical result from studies where actively pursuing happiness ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "apparently ends up decreasing it", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The importance of social networks for innovation and productivity", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Economies today are increasingly built on ideas. Consider, for example, commercial aviation. After a flight, ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Southwest Airlines ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "can unload and reload passengers to get the same airliner back in the air in about 10 minutes \u2013 this is half the time the same process took in the 1970s. As the economist John Cochrane ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://review.chicagobooth.edu/economics/2019/article/what-success-rock-climbing-tells-us-about-economic-growth", "children": [ { "text": "explains", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", this achievement is as much about ideas and know-how as it is about technology. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Southwest Airlines", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", and many other carriers after them, have been able to expand their output because they now have ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.npr.org/2015/06/28/418147961/the-man-who-saved-southwest-airlines-with-a-10-minute-idea", "children": [ { "text": "more efficient protocols", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " for \u2018turning planes\u2019 between flights.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In 2018 Paul Romer won the Nobel Prize in Economics \u201cfor integrating technological innovations into long-run macroeconomic analysis\u201d. Romer first fleshed out this observation in his ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "seminal 1990 paper", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", titled \u201cEndogenous Technological Change\u201d. At the heart of his contributions was precisely this observation: the diffusion of ideas fosters economic and productivity growth.{ref}Romer\u2019s seminal paper, with nearly 30,000 citations in Google Scholar is Romer, P. M. (1990). Endogenous technological change. Journal of Political Economy, 98 (5, Part 2), S71-S102. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". After this paper he wrote many other related articles elaborating on the same ideas. See, for example, Romer, P. M. (1992). Two strategies for economic development: using ideas and producing ideas. The World Bank Economic Review, 6(suppl_1), 63-91. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://academic.oup.com/wber/article-abstract/6/suppl_1/63/1656015?redirectedFrom=fulltext", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Romer\u2019s theory of \u201cendogenous growth\u201d \u2013 placing ideas at the heart of economic technologies \u2013 made economists ask entirely new questions about the drivers of growth. Where do ideas come from? How do ideas spread?\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Economists have asked these questions of the data, and their findings suggest social networks play a key role. Friendship and professional ties matter for economic growth because people tend to develop new ideas by interacting and learning from others who are close to them.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In a nutshell, social networks facilitate the diffusion of ideas across individuals and firms, and because of this, they play an important role in productivity growth. Here\u2019s how this works.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Knowledge spillovers, social networks, and patent citations", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Economic theory suggests we should expect social connections to have an effect on productivity via \u201cknowledge spillovers\".\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The basic idea is that social connections make it easier for knowledge to travel farther and faster. Knowledge is different from many physical assets, in that it can be used by many people at the same time; so the diffusion of knowledge creates productivity gains that spillover through society. As Thomas Jefferson once said: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cHe who receives an idea from me, receives instruction himself without lessening mine; as he who lights his taper at mine, receives light without darkening me\u201d.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "{ref}Letter to Isaac McPherson. August 13, 1813. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://en.wikisource.org/wiki/Letter_to_Isaac_McPherson_-_August_13,_1813", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "What is the empirical value of this theory? What do \u2018knowledge spillovers\u2019 actually look like in the real world? Do scientists and engineers really rely on social connections to disseminate their new ideas and inventions?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "To answer these questions economists have looked into historical data from ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patent-applications-per-million", "children": [ { "text": "patents", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Patents are government licences used to register the intellectual property of new inventions, ranging ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patents-usa-by-category", "children": [ { "text": "from machines to medical drugs", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". When a patent application is filed, inventors are required to include a list of citations, in which they describe how their technological development builds upon the results of earlier patents. These historical records of patent citations give us an overview of how knowledge flows \u2013 patent citations are the \u2018paper trail\u2019 left by ideas as they travel, evolve, and make new ideas possible.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "One of the first analyses of patent citations in the context of knowledge spillovers was done by Adam Jaffe, Manuel Trajtenberg and Rebecca Henderson, in ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813", "children": [ { "text": "a much-cited paper", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " published in the Quarterly Journal of Economics in 1993.{ref}Jaffe, A. B., Trajtenberg, M., & Henderson, R. (1993). Geographic localization of knowledge spillovers as evidenced by patent citations. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 108(3), 577-598. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Jaffe and coauthors studied, in particular, the geographic distribution of citation patterns in the US.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "If social connections did not matter for new ideas and growth, then we should expect that new inventions are just as likely to cite patents from far away as they are to cite the work of their neighbours. In contrast, Jaffe and coauthors found that new patents were disproportionately more likely to cite previous ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "local", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " patents. In other words, they found that patent citations were strongly geographically concentrated. According to their estimates, citations were three to four times as likely to come from the same state as the originating patent \u2013 this is consistent with the idea that social connections and proximity do indeed matter for innovation.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "After this initial study, several other papers have used patent citations to explore the determinants of knowledge spillovers, with a broader perspective on \u2018proximity\u2019.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Ajay Agrawal, Devesh Kapur and John McHale used the ethnicity of researchers as a proxy for social connectedness, and ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.515.3876&rep=rep1&type=pdf", "children": [ { "text": "found", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that co-location and co-ethnicity both increase the probability of knowledge flow between inventors. Co-location increases the probability of a knowledge flow by 24% (assuming non-co-ethnic inventors) and co-ethnicity increases the probability by 14% (assuming non-co-located inventors). Again, these large concentration patterns seem consistent with the idea that proximity and social networks matter for innovation.{ref}\u00a0Agrawal, A., Kapur, D., & McHale, J. (2008). How do spatial and social proximity influence knowledge flows? Evidence from patent data. Journal of Urban Economics, 64(2), 258-269.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Disentangling geographic proximity and social interactions", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The correlation between proximity and patent citations partly captures the effect that local factors may have on innovation (e.g. geography, regulation or infrastructure around industry hubs where similar workers and firms concentrate). However, the evidence shows that social connections matter for the transmission of knowledge even after we control for these other factors.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259", "children": [ { "text": "a paper", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " published in 2018 in the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Journal of Economic Perspectives", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", Michael Bailey, Rachel Cao, Theresa Kuchler, Johannes Stroebel, and Arlene Wong combined patent records with Facebook data from the US, and found that social connectedness between US counties (measured by the frequency of Facebook ties between counties) predicts the probability of cross-county patent citations. This is true, even after accounting for the patents\u2019 technology class and geographic distance between counties.{ref}\u00a0The full reference of the paper is: Bailey, M., Cao, R., Kuchler, T., Stroebel, J., & Wong, A. (2018). Social connectedness: measurement, determinants, and effects. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32(3), 259-80. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This means that the probability that an inventor builds on the work of another inventor in a different part of the country can be partly explained by how well-connected their social backgrounds are, even after controlling for geographic distance and other demographic factors.{ref}To be more precise, Bailey and co-authors find that the effect of doubling social connectedness on the probability of citation remains significant and large, at 4.9 percent, even after controlling for geographic distance. When they add controls for cross-country differences on socioeconomic indicators (income, education, ethnicity, etc.) they actually find that the estimated relationship between social connectedness and patent citation increases somewhat as a result of these further controls. This finding suggests that the relationship between geographic distance and the probability of patent citation, viewed in isolation, may be partially capturing effects of information flows associated with social connectedness.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The implications of this result are of great consequence in the age of the internet. The development of new communication-enabling technologies, such as ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media", "children": [ { "text": "the rise of social media", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", have opened new possibilities for creating and maintaining social connections, and this is likely to have had a positive effect on the transmission of knowledge.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Social connections and (material) well-being", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Globally, the total economic output per person today is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&tab=chart&year=1950&time=1870..latest&country=OWID_WRL", "children": [ { "text": "4.4-times larger", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " than in 1950 (even after accounting for inflation). This was achieved over a period during which people tended towards working ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/annual-working-hours-per-worker?tab=chart&stackMode=absolute&time=1950..2017&country=BRA~COL~FRA~DEU~NLD~GBR~USA®ion=World", "children": [ { "text": "fewer hours", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". We now produce more whilst working less.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "As our interactive chart here shows, this historical achievement is underpinned by productivity growth: in many countries, workers produce much higher output per hour today than in 1950.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This is partly because of new machines and technologies, but also because we have new shared knowledge about how to work more efficiently.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The research on social connections and innovation suggests that one important way to improve material living standards is to invest in digital communication technologies. This lowers the costs of creating and maintaining personal and professional ties, which facilitates the diffusion of ideas and knowledge, creating positive productivity spillovers.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/labor-productivity-per-hour-PennWorldTable?time=1950..2017&country=BRA+DEU+IND+JPN+KOR+GBR+USA", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "gray-section", "items": [ { "text": [ { "text": "Additional information", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "expandable-paragraph", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Social networks are diverse and complex, so talking about \u201csocial connectedness\u201d is actually a simplification that deserves some discussion. Researchers studying social networks have shown that the flow of information and knowledge through people is not only a function of \u201cconnectedness\u201d as measured by the number of links people have, but also a function of the specific structures of the networks (e.g. the extent to which social ties are tightly clustered). ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "For the diffusion of information, the concept of \u2018homophily\u2019, for example, is key. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "Homophily is the tendency of agents to associate with other agents who have similar characteristics. This manifests itself in many ways \u2013 for example, in the fact that friendship ties are typically strongly related to ethnicity. If one is interested in how information spreads through social learning, patterns such as homophily or social segregation are important to understand. For example, in the labor market, homophily isolates workers of one ethnicity from workers of another ethnicity, which then limits the extent to which individuals in one group hear about openings and opportunities known to the other group.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "Social networks research in this area shows that, in general, as homophily increases, the propensity for a diffusion to gain hold within a particular group rises, sometimes at the expense of the speed and extent of diffusion throughout the entire population. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Similarly, diversity may play a role. Observational studies ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://science.sciencemag.org/content/328/5981/1029", "children": [ { "text": "show", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that the diversity of individuals\u2019 relationships is strongly correlated with the economic development of communities.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "You can read more about this in Jackson, M. O., Rogers, B. W., & Zenou, Y. (2017). The economic consequences of social-network structure. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 49-95. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.econstor.eu/bitstream/10419/175053/1/Wp1116.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, West German families that had relatives in East Germany saw an increase in their income, and this growth in incomes was ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "significantly higher", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " than that of comparable West German families without such family ties to the East.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Why did this happen? Are personal and family ties really so important to our economic outcomes that they result in systematic differences in income growth?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Researchers have studied this question and have found empirical support for the idea that social connections do, indeed, have large effects on economic outcomes.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "A natural experiment: Friends and incomes after the fall of the Berlin Wall", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Measuring the impact of personal relations on economic outcomes is difficult. This is because these two aspects of our life are often closely intertwined \u2013 social ties facilitate economic interactions, but at the same time people with whom we interact for economic reasons end up becoming acquaintances or even friends.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "To overcome this difficulty, the economists Konrad Burchardi and Tarek Hassan studied a historical event that suddenly opened up new economic interactions, where previously only non-economic social connections were possible: The fall of the Berlin Wall.{ref}Konrad B. Burchardi and Tarek A. Hassan. \"The economic impact of social ties: Evidence from German reunification.\" The Quarterly Journal of Economics 128.3 (2013): 1219-1271. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "For decades private economic exchange between East and West Germany was virtually impossible, but at the same time people on both sides maintained social ties for non-economic reasons. After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, trade and collaboration between the two Germanies suddenly became feasible. Burchardi and Hassan studied the impact that this sudden change had on household incomes.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989 there was ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&time=1500..2018&country=~DEU", "children": [ { "text": "growth in incomes", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " across Germany as a whole; but the interesting finding from Burchardi and Hassan is that income growth for households in West Germany who had ties to at least one relative in the East was much higher \u2013 six percentage points higher \u2013 than that of comparable households without such ties.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Burchardi and Hassan argue that West German households that had ties to East Germany had a comparative advantage in seizing the new economic opportunities in the East. Having personal relationships with East Germans gave them access to valuable economic information \u2013 information regarding local demand conditions, and about the quality of East German assets that were offered to investors.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "These gains from social connections actually added up at the regional level. West German regions that had a higher concentration of households with social ties to the East, enjoyed substantially higher growth in incomes in the early 1990s. A one standard deviation rise in the share of households with social ties to East Germany in 1989 was associated with a 4.6 percentage point rise in income per capita over six years.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Personal relations and job search: Evidence from Europe", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The unique situation of the German reunification shows that social ties that are initially unrelated to economic interests can become important for economic success.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The key mechanism is that personal connections allow us to seize new economic opportunities. This may sound abstract, but in fact most of us have experienced this in our own lives. Perhaps the best example is job searching: personal contacts and connections are one of the most common ways for people to find a job.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the book ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "The Human Network", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", the economist Matthew Jackson draws on decades of research to help explain how social connections affect power, beliefs and behaviors. With regard to job networks and social capital, he writes: \u201c", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "If you have ever struggled to find a job, you are not alone. Without well-connected friends or family it is hard to make it in any industry [...]. Finding a job without having some personal connection to someone already employed at a firm is more the exception than the rule.\u201d ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "{ref}Pages 239-241 in Jackson, M. O. (2019). ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "The Human Network: How Your Social Position Determines Your Power, Beliefs, and Behaviors", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ". Pantheon.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Our interactive chart here shows evidence for Jackson\u2019s claim using data from European countries.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart shows the distribution of job search methods by country. Data for Spain is shown by default, but you can explore data for different countries using the option \u2018Change country\u2019.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In Spain, 84% of the unemployed rely on contacts \u2013 friends, relatives, trade unions \u2013 to find a job. It is the most common job-search method. Direct applications and ads rank below, on places two and three.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "If you plot the same data for other countries you\u2019ll see that personal contacts are a very important job-search method everywhere in Europe, but there is some variation across countries. At the lowest end of the spectrum, in Belgium, about 38% of people rely on contacts to find a job; at the other end, in Greece and Latvia, 90% of job seekers rely on this method.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "(NB. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-friends-relatives-trade-unions-in-job-search?tab=map", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "This interactive map", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "children": [ { "text": " shows the same data, but focusing specifically on the proportion of job seekers relying on contacts for job search across Europe.)", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-specific-search-methods", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Friends and relatives remain a popular method for finding a job, even in the age of digital job search platforms\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart showing job search methods across Europe relies on data from the European Labor Force Survey, 2006-2008. We have not found more recent data that allows cross-country comparisons, but surveys from individual high-income countries show that this remains relevant today, even after the expansion of online job search engines, and ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media", "children": [ { "text": "the rise of social media platforms", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the US, data from the Pew Research Center shows that the internet is the most important resource for job search, but 66% of survey respondents still say they relied on connections with close friends or family in their most recent search for a job.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "These two methods are of course not mutually exclusive, and the boundaries between \u2018internet\u2019 and \u2018personal contact\u2019 are becoming less clear as job search platforms become more integrated with social media.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/job-seeker-job-search-pew", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Personal relations matter for trade", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Personal relations can also provide information and social collateral that can be valuable for economic interactions across national borders.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The political scientist Anna Lee Saxenian ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "analyzed", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " the biographies of Chinese and Indian engineers who migrated to California in the 1970s. She found that as these skilled immigrants excelled in their personal careers, they started leveraging their social ties to relatives and friends in their home regions. This allowed them to outsource important operations for a large fraction of Silicon Valley firms.{ref}Saxenian, A. (2000). Silicon Valley\u2019s new immigrant entrepreneurs. UC San DiegoWorking Papers. The Center for Comparative Immigration Studies. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Saxenian argues that by connecting Silicon Valley firms to low-cost, high-quality labor in India and China, they became instrumental for the success of Silicon Valley and crucial in the emergence of their home regions as major hubs of the global IT services industry.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Personal relations matter for ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "collective", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " economic outcomes", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In some situations personal connections may facilitate economic transactions for one person (or group of people) at the expense of another one. This is what many people have in mind when they think about job referrals. If there is only one job vacancy, giving the job to a friend means not giving the job to someone else. If social connections become a vehicle for favouritism, this can lead to inefficiencies.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In many contexts, however, the opposite is true. Social connections often have a net-positive effect on aggregate economic outcomes because much of the impact of social connections on economic transactions is not zero-sum \u2013 for example, when social connections reduce information frictions and allow people to learn from each other. As we discuss in a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-networks-innovation-and-productivity", "children": [ { "text": "companion post", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", there is empirical evidence showing these knowledge spillovers are substantial.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Social networks can affect economic outcomes via agglomeration effects", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The concentration of firms and workers in \u2018industry hubs\u2019 is partly explained by the fact that social connections create economic externalities: Agglomeration leads to denser social connections, and this has positive spillover effects on productivity.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Again the fall of the Berlin Wall offers clear evidence of this.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "When Berlin was split, economic activity in West Berlin shifted away from the historic business district, known as \u2018Mitte\u2019. As a consequence property prices there fell relative to the new economic areas that emerged further away from the Wall.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "When the Wall came down the distribution of economic activity within the city changed quickly once again: The old business district began to re-emerge and economic activity shifted back to the historic center. This is the conclusion of a careful econometric study, published by Gabriel Ahlfeldt, Stephen Redding, Daniel Sturm and Nikolaus Wolf in the journal ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Econometrica", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ".{ref}\u00a0Ahlfeldt, G. M., Redding, S. J., Sturm, D. M., & Wolf, N. (2015). The economics of density: Evidence from the Berlin Wall. Econometrica, 83(6), 2127-2189. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.3982/ECTA10876", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The three maps show the evolution of land prices in Berlin. These maps, from Ahlfeldt and co-authors, illustrate graphically the impact that the Wall had on the geography of economic activity. The vertical scale measures relative land prices; this means higher elevation, highlighted with darker shades, denotes higher prices.{ref}\u00a0The authors use the same vertical scale for each figure, and land prices are normalized to have a mean of 1 in each year, so the levels of the land price surfaces in each map are comparable.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The map on the left shows that in 1936 there are two areas with very high land values in West Berlin and both are close to the center (they are both part of a concentric ring around the pre-war central business district, located in the middle).", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In 1986, we see smaller peak values; in fact, we see that the pre-war peak closest to the Wall is entirely eliminated following division, as this area ceased to be an important center of commercial and retail activity.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Finally, in 2006, we see again higher peaks; and we see that the area closest to the Wall is reemerging as a center of high land values.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This evolution is a great example of the economics of density: The areas in West Berlin close to the old central business district became less productive during the division but regained their productivity and attractiveness again soon after reunification.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Knowledge, public resources, and opportunities for trade and collaboration that had been inaccessible for decades, suddenly became available after the fall of the Wall.{ref}\u00a0Ahlfeldt and coauthors disentangle agglomeration effects from other forces that took place at the same time. In particular, they account for the fact that the Berlin Wall had an impact both on agglomeration forces (i.e. \u201cproduction externalities\u201d determined by the surrounding density of workers); as well as on \"production fundamentals\" (i.e. the physical characteristics of the different neighborhoods where workers and firms concentrate).{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "alt": "", "size": "wide", "type": "image", "filename": "berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015.png", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Social connections matter hugely for economic outcomes. Personal relations, even those that we maintain for non-economic reasons, often give us access to information and provide us with social collateral for economic transactions \u2013 from buying a house to getting a job.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This can have positive consequences that extend beyond the individuals directly involved in the transactions. When personal and professional ties facilitate the exchange of information and knowledge, this can bring benefits to society as a whole.\u00a0The implication from this is that social connections are not only important because they ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being", "children": [ { "text": "affect our emotional well-being", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", but also because, as we show here, they affect our ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "material", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " well-being too.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Loneliness, solitude and social isolation", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 1, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Living alone is becoming increasingly common around the world", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the US, the share of adults who live alone nearly doubled over the last 50 years. This is not only happening in the US: single-person households have become increasingly common in many countries across the world, from Angola to Japan.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Historical records show that this \u2018rise of living alone\u2019 started in early-industrialized countries over a century ago, accelerating around 1950. In countries such as Norway and Sweden, single-person households were rare a century ago, but today they account for nearly half of all households. In some cities they are already the majority.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Surveys and census data from recent decades show that people are more likely to live alone in rich countries, and the prevalence of single-person households is unprecedented historically.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Social connections \u2013 including contact with friends and family \u2013 are ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being", "children": [ { "text": "important", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as single-person households become more common, there will be new challenges to connect and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where welfare states are weaker.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "But it\u2019s important to keep things in perspective. It\u2019s unhelpful to compare the rise of living alone with a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019, which is what newspaper articles often write in ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA", "children": [ { "text": "alarming headlines", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Loneliness and solitude are ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone", "children": [ { "text": "not the same", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", and the evidence suggests that self-reported loneliness has ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "not", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " been growing", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " in recent decades.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Historical perspective on people living alone: Evidence from rich countries\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Historical records of inhabitants across villages and cities in today\u2019s rich countries give us insights into how uncommon it was for people to live alone in the past.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart here, adapted from ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true", "children": [ { "text": "a paper", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " by the historian Keith Snell, shows estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data. Each dot corresponds to an estimate for one settlement in Europe, North America, Japan or Britain.{ref}Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The share of one-person households remained fairly steady between the early modern period and through the 19th century \u2013 typically below 10%. Then growth started in the twentieth century, accelerating in the 1960s.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The current prevalence of one-person households is unprecedented historically. The highest point recorded in this chart corresponds to Stockholm, in 2012, where 60% of households consist of one person.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "alt": "", "size": "wide", "type": "image", "filename": "historical-one-person-households.png", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The rise of one-person households across the world", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "For recent decades, census data can be combined with data from large cross-country surveys, to provide a global perspective on the proportion of households with only one member (i.e. the proportion of single-person households). This gives us a proxy for the prevalence of solitary living arrangements.{ref}The percentage of single-person households is a proxy for \u2018solitary living\u2019. However, it\u2019s important to emphasise that these two things are not really the same. Single-person households include those where a person lives alone in an individual housing unit, but they also include people who live independently as lodgers in a separate room within a larger housing unit with other occupant. So technically, there are some people who live in a \u2018single-person household\u2019, but they don\u2019t really live alone.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "We produced this chart combining individual reports from statistical country offices, cross-country surveys such as the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://dhsprogram.com/", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Demographic and Health Surveys", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", and estimates published in the EU\u2019s ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "url": "https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat", "children": [ { "text": "Eurostat", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", the UN\u2019s ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://unstats.un.org/unsd/demographic-social/products/dyb/", "children": [ { "text": "Demographic Year Books", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", and the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://figshare.com/articles/German_Time_Series_Dataset_1834_2012/1450809/1", "children": [ { "text": "Deutschland in Daten", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " dataset. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart shows that the trend of rising single-person households extends across all world regions. There are large differences between countries \u2013 from more than 40% in northern European countries to 1% in low-income Asian countries.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "(NB. For the US and Canada there are long-run time series from census data that let us directly track the share of people who live alone. This is shown in ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/adults-living-alone", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "this other chart", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "children": [ { "text": ", where you can see the same trend.) ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households", "children": [], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Living arrangements and prosperity", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "National income per capita and the share of one-person households are strongly correlated: As the chart here shows, people are more likely to live alone in rich countries.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In this interactive chart you can move the slider to see changes over time. This reveals that the rise of single-person households tends to be larger in countries where GDP per capita has grown more. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "(NB. You can also see the correlation over time in ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?stackMode=relative&time=1990..2016", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "this other", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "children": [ { "text": " scatter plot comparing average growth in GDP vs average growth in one-person households).", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "These correlations are partly due to the fact that people who can afford to, often choose to live alone. Indeed, rising incomes in many countries are likely part of the reason why people are more likely to live alone today than in the past.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "But there must be more to it since even at the same level of incomes there are clear differences between regions. In particular, Asian countries have systematically fewer one-person households than African countries with comparable GDP levels. Ghana and Pakistan, for example, have similar GDP per capita, but in Pakistan one-person households are extremely rare, while in Ghana they are common (about 1 in 4). This suggests culture and country-specific factors also play an important role.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Additionally, there are other non-cultural country-specific factors that are likely to play a role. In particular, rich countries often have more extensive social support networks, so people in these countries find it easier to take risks. Living alone is more risky in poorer countries, because there\u2019s often less supply of services and infrastructure to support more solitary living arrangements.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "And finally, it\u2019s also likely that some of the causality runs in the opposite direction. It\u2019s not only that incomes, culture or welfare states enable people to live alone, but also that for many workers attaining higher incomes in today\u2019s economy often demands changes in living arrangements. Migration", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/urbanization", "children": [ { "text": " from rural to urban areas", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " is the prime example.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Is the rise of one-person households a problem?\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Social connections \u2013 including contact with friends and family \u2013 are ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being", "children": [ { "text": "important", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as the \u2018rise of living alone\u2019 continues, there will be new challenges to connect people and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where communication technologies are ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/internet", "children": [ { "text": "less developed", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " and welfare states are ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/government-spending#high-income-countries-spend-more-on-social-protection-than-low-income-countries", "children": [ { "text": "weaker", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "But, it\u2019s also important to keep in mind that living alone is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone", "children": [ { "text": "not the same", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " as feeling lonely. There\u2019s evidence that living alone is, by itself, a poor predictor of loneliness. Self-reported loneliness has ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic", "children": [ { "text": "not been growing ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": "in recent decades, and in fact, the countries where people are most likely to say they have support from family and friends, are the same countries \u2013 in Scandinavia \u2013 where a large fraction of the population lives alone.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Incomes and freedom of choice are not the only drivers of the \u2018rise of living alone\u2019; but it would be remiss to ignore they do contribute to this trend.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/economic-growth", "children": [ { "text": "Higher incomes", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/structural-transformation-and-deindustrialization-evidence-from-todays-rich-countries", "children": [ { "text": "economic transitions", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that enable migration from agriculture in rural areas into manufacturing and services in cities, and ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/female-labor-supply", "children": [ { "text": "rising female participation in labor markets", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " all play a role. People are more likely to live alone today than in the past partly because they are increasingly able to do so.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Loneliness and aloneness are not the same", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There is a popular ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.vice.com/sv/article/mvn7eb/the-swedish-theory-of-love-or-why-we-swedes-are-so-lonely-234", "children": [ { "text": "perception", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that countries in Northern Europe are heavily individualistic and because of this people in these societies tend to be much lonelier. The data, however, does not support this claim.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "What is true is that in countries such as Denmark and Switzerland, it is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE", "children": [ { "text": "very common", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " for people to live alone. But contrary to what many believe, this does not translate into higher levels of self-reported loneliness.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Below we discuss the data in more detail, and show that this can be partly explained by the fact that loneliness and aloneness are just not the same. Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it\u2019s important not to conflate them.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Self-reported loneliness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Loneliness describes a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "subjective", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " feeling; this is conceptually distinct from ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "objective", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " physical isolation.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the chart here we show estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness among older adults. The data comes from various surveys asking people directly whether they often experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cI have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ").", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The differences in the prevalence of loneliness across countries are very large. At the bottom of the list, Denmark, Switzerland, Sweden and the US all have rates below 30%, while at the top of the list Greece, Israel and Italy all have rates of close to or above 50%.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The data is available only for fifteen countries, but this sample does not suggest that ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Individualistic_culture", "children": [ { "text": "'rich individualistic societies'", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " are lonelier than others. In fact, at the very bottom are two countries in which the share of people living alone is among the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE", "children": [ { "text": "highest", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " in the world.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults", "children": [], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Perceptions of social support", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Similar to loneliness, we can measure perceptions of social support by asking people directly. This is what the polling organization ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Gallup", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " did in their flagship ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.gallup.com/analytics/232838/world-poll.aspx", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "World Poll", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " survey. Specifically, they asked: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cIf you were in trouble, do you have relatives or friends you can count on to help you whenever you need them, or not?\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart here presents the results from this survey, plotting the share of people who responded \u201cyes\u201d to this question.{ref}\u00a0The data for this chart comes from a table published in the appendix of the OECD report \u201cHow's Life? 2017 Measuring Well-Being\u201d, available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Differences across countries are not very large. The lowest and highest average levels of support corresponds to Mexico and Iceland, at 80% and 98% respectively.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The second point that stands out is that, again, there\u2019s no support for the claim that richer countries that are considered to be more individualistic (e.g. North European countries) have lower levels of family and friendship support.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/people-who-report-having-friends-or-relatives-they-can-count-on", "type": "chart", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Distinguishing between aloneness and loneliness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Are people who live alone more likely to say they feel lonely?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The researchers Caitlin Coyle and Elizabeth Dugan explored this question using data from the", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": " Health and Retirement Study", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " in the US. In their ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275", "children": [ { "text": "analysis", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", covering almost 12,000 respondents over the years 2006-2008, they found that loneliness and social isolation are not highly correlated at the individual level.{ref}Coyle, C. E., & Dugan, E. (2012). Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. Journal of aging and health, 24(8), 1346-1363. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "To be precise, Coyle and Dugan found a correlation coefficient of 0.2. That means the correlation is positive, and it is statistically larger than zero; but it\u2019s not very large in absolute terms. Their study does not provide the raw data, but in general, a scatter plot with correlation 0.2 is pretty much just a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Correlation_and_dependence", "children": [ { "text": "cloud of dots", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This means you cannot predict much of the variability in loneliness from observed differences in living arrangements. Some people who live alone are lonely, but many are not. Concluding that someone who lives alone must feel lonely is very often wrong.{ref}There\u2019s similar evidence from other countries. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Community Life Survey", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", in which they ask about living arrangements and loneliness. This survey finds that while the reported frequency of loneliness is certainly higher among those who live alone, the correlation is not higher that of other \u2018risk factors\u2019 such as disability, age and marital status. You find more information in the ONS report ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Being alone and feeling lonely are not the same", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "People do not seem to be more lonely in societies that are ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.hofstede-insights.com/country-comparison/finland,norway,sweden/", "children": [ { "text": "traditionally labeled", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " as 'individualistic'.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "What is true is that in these societies it is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE", "children": [ { "text": "particularly common", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " for people to live alone. But being alone and feeling lonely don\u2019t always go hand in hand. Many people feel lonely even if they are not physically isolated; and many people who are physically isolated do not feel lonely.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The aggregate statistics confirm this. Surveys that ask people about living arrangements, time use, and feelings of loneliness, find that solitude, by itself, does not predict feelings of loneliness.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The fact that people in individualistic countries are not more likely to feel lonely may of course capture differences in expectations regarding social relations. But to the extent that loneliness is a subjective experience, these cross-country differences are still important to understand well-being.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it\u2019s important not to conflate them.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Changes in loneliness over time: Is there a loneliness epidemic?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The media seems to have agreed that rich countries are experiencing a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019. There are literally ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA", "children": [ { "text": "thousands", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " of newspaper articles that use this exact expression.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "What is the evidence for this? The word \u2018epidemic\u2019 suggests that things are getting much worse and loneliness is increasing rapidly. But does the data in fact show that societies are becoming lonelier?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Despite the popularity of the claim, there is surprisingly no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "It is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone", "children": [ { "text": "true", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that more people are living alone around the world. But loneliness and aloneness are not the same. As we explain in a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone", "children": [ { "text": "companion post", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", spending time alone is not a good predictor of whether people feel lonely, or have weaker social support.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "As we explain later, today\u2019s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today\u2019s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than older adults in the past. Surveys covering older adults in other rich countries, including Finland, Germany, England and Sweden, point in the same direction \u2013 it\u2019s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations in these countries.{ref}\u00a0See the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Social connections \u2013 including contact with friends and family \u2013 are important for our ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being", "children": [ { "text": "health and emotional welfare", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", as well as for our ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/personal-relations-econ-outcomes", "children": [ { "text": "material well-being", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". Loneliness is indeed an important problem, but it\u2019s crucial to have a nuanced conversation based on facts. The headlines that claim we are witnessing a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019 are wrong and unhelpful.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Are young people more lonely than older adults?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "One statistic that is often used to argue that loneliness is increasing, is that young people today are lonelier than older adults. This begs two questions: (i) Is it true that younger people are lonelier, and (ii) does this show that loneliness is increasing?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Let\u2019s start with the first question. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/community-life-survey-2018-19", "children": [ { "text": "Community Life Survey", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", in which they ask people how often they feel lonely. In the bar chart here we show a breakdown of self-reported loneliness by age group.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "According to this data, those aged 16 to 24 are the group most likely to report feeling lonely, with 10% feeling lonely \u201coften or always\u201d. In contrast, those aged 65 years and older are the group least likely to report feeling lonely, with 3% feeling lonely \u201coften or always\u201d.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Many people tend to associate loneliness with older age, so this pattern might seem surprising. But surveys from several other rich countries have found the same. In ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://archive.stats.govt.nz/browse_for_stats/people_and_communities/older_people/loneliness-in-nz-2010-NZGSS/loneliness-in-nz.aspx", "children": [ { "text": "New Zealand", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://files.kff.org/attachment/Report-Loneliness-and-Social-Isolation-in-the-United-States-the-United-Kingdom-and-Japan-An-International-Survey", "children": [ { "text": "Japan", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " and the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.multivu.com/players/English/8294451-cigna-us-loneliness-survey/docs/IndexReport_1524069371598-173525450.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "US", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", young adults also report feeling lonely more often than older adults.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "So, yes, in rich countries we find that younger people are more likely to report feeling lonely.{ref}The emphasis on \u2018rich countries\u2019 is important. A study exploring self-reported loneliness across nine former Soviet countries, found that older adults report higher levels of loneliness. This shows it's wrong to generalize from the available data, which is primarily from rich countries. For more details see: Stickley, A., Koyanagi, A., Roberts, B., Richardson, E., Abbott, P., Tumanov, S., & McKee, M. (2013). Loneliness: its correlates and association with health behaviours and outcomes in nine countries of the former Soviet Union. PloS one, 8(7), e67978. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "What about the second question? Does this mean that loneliness is increasing?\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Here, the answer is \u2018no\u2019. Cross-sectional comparisons are not informative about changes ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "over time", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle. To be able to say something meaningful about changes in loneliness over time, we need to distinguish between changes for ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "individuals", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " over time (do people become lonelier as they get older?) and changes across generations (are people of the same age lonelier today than in the past?).", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Let\u2019s dig deeper and explore these questions separately.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "alt": "", "size": "wide", "type": "image", "filename": "Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England.png", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Do we become lonelier as we get older?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "To understand how loneliness changes across our life cycle, we need loneliness data from surveys that track the same individuals over time, up until old age. In a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download", "children": [ { "text": "study", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " published in the journal ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Psychology and Aging", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", Louise Hawkley and co-authors examine two such surveys, with data for adults older than 50 years in the US.{ref}\u00a0\u00a0Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "They found that after age 50 \u2013 which is the earliest age of participants in their study \u2013 loneliness tended to decrease, until about 75, after which it began to increase again.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The authors explain in their paper that the increase in loneliness after 75 was explained by a decline in health and the loss of a spouse or partner. When adjusting for these factors, they found that loneliness continued declining into \u2018oldest old age\u2019.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This shows that there are two forces at play. On the one hand, there seems to be a direct relationship between age and loneliness, whereby loneliness ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "decreases", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " with age as our social expectations adapt, and we become ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1207/S15324834BASP2304_2?casa_token=885gubc-kPAAAAAA:gP8oVDQ-085Ftuh1-pvmG4Ms4-O58KDka4hjgK5Nq7RjS3ZsESymhhPo_owFbaf5MufXSx2LvFc0oA", "children": [ { "text": "more selective", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " about relating with contacts who bring positive emotions. On the other hand, there seems to be an indirect association pushing in the opposite direction, whereby loneliness ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "increases", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " with age, because our health deteriorates and we lose relatives and friends.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In our middle age the direct effect dominates, but once we enter advanced old age, the negative indirect effect starts dominating.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This complex relationship between age and loneliness shows why comparing old and young people at a given point in time is misleading. Cross-sectional comparisons are just not informative about the evolution of loneliness over time, because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Loneliness trends over time: Are people lonelier today than in the past?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019 narrative, it is often implied that if we compare two individuals of the same age \u2013 one today and another one a generation ago \u2013 we would find that the one today is more likely to feel lonely. This is based on the idea that there have been societal changes \u2013 such as the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone", "children": [ { "text": "rise of living alone", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " \u2013 that make newer generations more likely to feel lonely.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In their ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download", "children": [ { "text": "study", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", Louise Hawkley and co-authors searched for evidence of these \u2018cohort trends\u2019 in the US, but didn\u2019t find any. There was very little difference in self-reported loneliness of people born in different generations. Those that were born in 1920-1947 experienced the same changes of loneliness throughout their lives as those born in 1948-1965. It\u2019s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "An ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://unherd.com/2019/05/is-the-epidemic-of-loneliness-fake-news/", "children": [ { "text": "article", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " by Tom Chivers explains this result from the study very well: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201c[Hawkley and co-authors] found that \u201cnewer\u201d old people (baby boomers born 1948-1965) are no more likely to think of themselves as lonely than \u201colder\u201d old people (born 1920-1947), and that older people have not become more likely to think of themselves as lonely in the last decade (2005 \u2013 2016)... The average older person appears to be no more likely to be lonely than they were a decade ago.\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There are studies with data from other rich countries pointing in the same direction. In Sweden, repeated cross-sectional surveys with adults aged 85, 90, and 95 years old, found no increase in loneliness over a ten-year interval.{ref}\u00a0Nyqvist, F., Cattan, M., Conradsson, M., N\u00e4sman, M., & Gustafsson, Y. (2017). Prevalence of loneliness over ten years among the oldest old. Scandinavian journal of public health, 45(4), 411-418. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1403494817697511", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". For similar results in other countries, including Finland, Germany and England, see the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "These studies find no evidence of cohort effects across older adults. What about evidence for adolescents?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the aptly titled ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789", "children": [ { "text": "paper", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " \u201cRethinking \u2018Generation Me\u2019: A Study of Cohort Effects from 1976-2006\u201d, the psychologists Kali Trzesniewski and Brent Donnellan used repeated cohort surveys to explore whether successive groups of high-school graduates were becoming more lonely in the US. They also found no evidence of cohort trends. Newer generations of high-school seniors were not more likely to report feelings of loneliness than earlier generations.{ref}\u00a0Trzesniewski and Donnellan relied on data from the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.monitoringthefuture.org", "children": [ { "text": "Monitoring the Future Project", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", an ongoing survey of young Americans. Their analysis extends beyond loneliness. The paper abstract explains the results as follows: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cSocial commentators have argued that changes over the last decades have coalesced to create a relatively unique generation of young people. However, using large samples of U.S. high-school seniors from 1976 to 2006 (Total N = 477,380), we found little evidence of meaningful change in egotism, self-enhancement, individualism, self-esteem, locus of control, hopelessness, happiness, life satisfaction, loneliness, antisocial behavior, time spent working or watching television, political activity, the importance of religion, and the importance of social status over the last 30 years.\u201d ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "The full reference of the paper is: Trzesniewski, K. H., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Rethinking \u201cGeneration Me\u201d: A study of cohort effects from 1976-2006. Perspectives on psychological science, 5(1), 58-75. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The psychologists Matthew Clark, Natalie Loxton, and Stephanie Tobin ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0146167214557007", "children": [ { "text": "replicated", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " this analysis, using the same survey, but focusing on all age groups, not only high-school seniors. The chart here shows their results. They found no signs of increasing loneliness across all age groups. In fact, they found a very small but statistically significant ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "decline in loneliness ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "for high school students in the US.{ref}\u00a0To measure loneliness, the project used a six-item questionnaire, where participants used a 1 (disagree) to 5 (agree) scale to respond to the following statements: \u2018\u2018", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "A lot of times I feel lonely", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ",\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "There is always someone I can turn to if I need help (reverse scored)", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ",\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "I often feel left out of things", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ",\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "There is usually someone I can talk to if I need to (reverse scored)", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ",\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "I often wish I had more good friends", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ",\u2019\u2019 and \u2018", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u2018I usually have a few good friends around that I can get together with (reverse scored).", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "\u2019\u2019 The project began collecting loneliness data for Grade 12 students in 1977, and data for Grades 8 and 10 in 1991.\u00a0{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "(NB. The vertical axis in this chart is truncated, following the presentation in the original paper. The truncated axis is helpful to highlight the trend; but the takeaway is that the changes in levels are extremely small, so the trend is effectively flat in absolute terms, even if the slope is statistically different from zero).", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "alt": "", "size": "wide", "type": "image", "filename": "declining-loneliness-of-US-students.png", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Loneliness deserves attention, but headlines that claim we are witnessing a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019 are not true, and are actually unhelpful", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The magazine ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "The Economist ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "wrote ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.economist.com/international/2018/09/01/loneliness-is-a-serious-public-health-problem", "children": [ { "text": "an article", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " in 2018 with the title \u201cLoneliness is a serious public-health problem\u201d. In one key paragraph, the article reads: \u201cHistorical data about loneliness are scant. But isolation does seem to be increasing, so loneliness may be too.\u201d\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The data we have found shows that this reasoning is actually incorrect.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Surveys from rich countries do not suggest there has been an increase in loneliness over time. Today\u2019s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today\u2019s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than did adults of their age in the past.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "That\u2019s of course not to say we should not pay attention to these topics.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "It\u2019s important to provide support to people who ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults", "children": [ { "text": "suffer from loneliness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", just as it is important to pay attention to the policy challenges that come from large societal changes such as the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone", "children": [ { "text": "rise of living alone", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". However, inaccurate, over-simplified narratives are unhelpful to really understand these complex challenges.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There is an epidemic of headlines that claim we are experiencing a \u201cloneliness epidemic\u201d, but there is no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Social media and social networks", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 1, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "The rise of social media platforms", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Are Facebook and other social media platforms bad for our well-being?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 2, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Facebook, Youtube, Whatsapp, WeChat, and Instagram are the top five social media platforms globally, with over ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/users-by-social-media-platform", "children": [ { "text": "one billion active users each", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ". In most rich countries the proportion of young people using online social networks ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/percentage-of-young-people-engaging-in-social-networking-online", "children": [ { "text": "exceeds 90%", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " and teens spend on average ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/daily-time-spent-on-the-internet-by-young-people", "children": [ { "text": "more than 4 hours online every day", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "We\u2019re repeatedly told in the news that social media is bad for us. The stories are often alarming, suggesting social media and smartphones are responsible for sweeping negative trends, ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression", "children": [ { "text": "from rising suicide rates in the US", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", to ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-families/social-media-mental-health-negative-effects-depression-anxiety-addiction-memory-a8307196.html", "children": [ { "text": "widespread loss in memory, and reduced sleep and attention spans", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "These worrying headlines often go together with implicit or explicit recommendations to limit the amount of time we spend on social media. Indeed, smartphones today come with built-in \u201cscreen time\u201d apps that let us track and limit how much time we spend online.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "At the same time, most of us would agree that digital social media platforms can make our lives easier in many ways \u2013 opening doors to new information, connecting us with people who are far away, and helping us to be more flexible with work.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "What does the research tell us about the causal impact of social media use on our well-being?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In a nutshell: From my reading of the scientific literature, I do not believe that the available evidence today supports the sweeping newspaper headlines.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Yes, there is evidence suggesting a causal negative effect, but the size of these causal effects is heterogeneous and much, much smaller than the news headlines suggest.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There are still plenty of good reasons to reflect on the impact of social media in society, and there is much we can all learn to make better use of these complex digital platforms. But this requires going beyond universal claims.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Let\u2019s take a look at the evidence.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Comparisons across individuals", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Most of the news stories that claim social media has a negative impact on well-being rely on data from surveys comparing individuals with different levels of social media use as evidence. In the chart, I show one concrete example of this type of correlational analysis.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart plots the average amount of time that people spend on social media each day, among people who are and aren\u2019t happy with the amount of time spent on these platforms.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The data comes from an app called ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://inthemoment.io/", "children": [ { "text": "Moment", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", which tracks the amount of time users spend on social media platforms on their smartphones. The app also asks people a yes/no question: \u201cAre you happy with your time spent?\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "As we can see there is quite a bit of heterogeneity across platforms, but the pattern is clear: People who say they are happy with how much time they spend on social media spend less time on these platforms. Or put differently, using social media more heavily is correlated with less satisfaction.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This is certainly interesting, but we should be careful not to jump to conclusions \u2013 the correlation actually raises as many questions as it answers.{ref}A concrete example of how this correlational evidence is reported in the news can be found in ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.economist.com/graphic-detail/2018/05/18/how-heavy-use-of-social-media-is-linked-to-mental-illness", "children": [ { "text": "this article from The Economist", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", where a very similar chart is presented under the headline \u201cHow heavy use of social media is linked to mental illness\u201d.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "alt": "", "size": "wide", "type": "image", "filename": "TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-\u2013-happy-vs-unhappy-users.png", "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Does this pattern hold if we control for user characteristics like age and gender? Would we get similar results if we focused on other measures of well-being beyond \u2018happy with time spent\u2019?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The answer to both questions is \u2018no\u2019. Depending on what outcome variables you focus on, and depending on which demographic characteristics you account for, you will get a different result. It is therefore not surprising that some empirical academic studies have found negative correlations; while others actually report positive correlations.{ref}Here are some examples of studies reporting a positive correlation between social media use and subjective well-being: - Kim, J. and Lee, J. (2011). The facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number of face- book friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. CyberPsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(6):359\u2013364 - Gonzales, A. L. and Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my facebook wall: Effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(1-2):79\u201383. - Valenzuela, S., Park, N., and Kee, K. F. (2009). Is there social capital in a social network site?: Facebook use and college students\u2019 life satisfaction, trust, and participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14(4):875\u2013901.{/ref} ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-018-0506-1", "children": [ { "text": "published a paper", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " earlier this year in the journal ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Nature", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " where they illustrated that given the flexibility to analyze the data (i.e. given the number of possible choices researchers have when it comes to processing and interpreting the vast data from these large surveys), scientists ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://socialsciences.nature.com/users/200472-amy-orben/posts/42763-beyond-cherry-picking", "children": [ { "text": "could have written thousands of papers", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " describing positive, negative and non-significant associations. Different ways of measuring well-being and social media use will yield different results, even for the same population.{ref}\u00a0It\u2019s also important to mention that the correlations between social media use and wellbeing are not generally comparable across countries and time. For example, commentators", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression", "children": [ { "text": " often highlight", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " that depression and suicide rates among adolescents in the US have been going up ever since the rise of smartphone, and that this is a clear red flag. Yet as Andrew Przybylski explained ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://rationallyspeakingpodcast.org/show/rs-237-andy-przybylski-on-is-screen-time-bad-for-you.html", "children": [ { "text": "in a recent interview", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", this is just not a general observation: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cYou don't see things like it in more tech-saturated countries, or in other industrialized countries. You don't see, two or three years ahead of the United States, the South Koreans and the Japanese having spiking rates of self-harm or depression\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Even the answers to some of the most fundamental questions are unclear:\u00a0 Do we actually know in which direction the relationship might be going? Does frequent social media use translate into lower happiness, or is it the other way around \u2013 are anxious, stressed or depressed people particularly prone to use social media?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This takes us to another branch of the literature: longitudinal studies that track individuals over time to measure changes in social media use and well-being.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Studies of social media use and well-being over time", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "One longitudinal study that has received much attention on this subject was ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143", "children": [ { "text": "published", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " by Holly Shakya and Nicholas Christakis in the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "American Journal of Epidemiology", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " in 2017. It used data from a survey that tracked a group of 5,208 Americans over the period 2013 - 2015, and found an increase in Facebook activity was associated with a future ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "decrease", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " in reported mental health.{ref}The full reference is: Shakya, H. B., & Christakis, N. A. (2017). Association of Facebook use with compromised well-being: A longitudinal study. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "American journal of epidemiology", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "185", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "(3), 203-211. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Two years later, Amy Orben, Tobias Dienlin and Andrew Przybylski ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "published a paper", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " in the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " using a similar source of data. They relied on a longitudinal survey from the UK covering 12,672 teenagers over the period 2009 - 2016, and reached a different conclusion. They found that there was a small and reciprocal relationship: social media use predicted small decreases in life satisfaction; but it was also the case that decreasing life satisfaction predicted subsequent increases in social media use.{ref}The full reference is: Orben, A., Dienlin, T., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Social media\u2019s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(21), 10226-10228. Online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Summarizing their research in ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "The Guardian", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2019/jul/07/too-much-screen-time-hurts-kids-where-is-evidence", "children": [ { "text": "explained", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ": ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "\u201cwe did find some small trends over time \u2013 these were mostly clustered in data provided by teenage girls\u2026 But \u2013 and this is key \u2013 it\u2019s not an exaggeration to say that these effects were minuscule by the standards of science and trivial if you want to inform personal parenting decisions. Our results indicated that 99.6% of the variability in adolescent girls\u2019 satisfaction with life had nothing to do with how much they used social media.\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In their paper Orben and co-authors argue again that these large datasets allow many different types of empirical tests; so it is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://slatestarcodex.com/2014/12/12/beware-the-man-of-one-study/", "children": [ { "text": "natural to expect conflicting results across studies", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", particularly if there is noise in measurement and the true effect sizes are small.{ref}What\u2019s more, if the cutoff for a statistically significant finding is 5% and you test 100 independent hypotheses of which in reality none are true, you will (in expectation) still find 5 significant associations in either direction simply by chance. Here is a good example and discussion of how this works: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1", "children": [ { "text": "https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": " Orben and co-authors tested thousands of empirical tests and indeed, some of these tests could have been interpreted on their own as evidence of a strong negative effect for social media \u2013 but clearly the broader picture is important. When looking at the results from all their thousands of tests, they concluded that social media effects were nuanced, small at best and reciprocal over time.{ref}One more piece of evidence that confirms this conclusion, comes from a more recent long-run study that tracked 500 adolescents for eight consecutive years, between the ages of 13 and 20, and found that ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": " \"increased time spent on social media was not associated with increased mental health issues across development.\"", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": " The full reference is Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L., & Booth, M. (2019). Does time spent using social media impact mental health?: An eight year longitudinal study.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "Computers in Human Behavior", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ", 106160. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0747563219303723", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Facebook experiments", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Establishing causal impacts through observational studies that track the well-being of individuals over time is difficult.\u00a0", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "First, there are measurement issues. Long-run surveys that track people are expensive and impose a high burden on participants, so they do not allow in-depth high-frequency data collection, and instead focus on broad trends across a wide range of topics. Orben and co-authors, for example, rely on the ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.understandingsociety.ac.uk/", "children": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Understanding Society Survey", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " from the UK, which covers a wide range of themes such as family life, education, employment, finance, health and wellbeing. Specifically on social media use, this survey only ask how many hours teenagers remember using apps during normal weekdays, which is of course an informative but noisy measure of actual use (a fact that Orben and co-authors mention in their paper).", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Second, there are limitations from unobservable variables. Frequent users of social media are likely different from less frequent users in ways that are hard to measure \u2013 no matter how many questions you include in a survey, there will always be relevant factors you cannot account for in the analysis.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Given these limitations, an obvious alternative is to run an experiment: you can, for example, offer people money to stop using Facebook for a while and then check the effect by comparing these \u201ctreated participants\u201d against a control group that is allowed to continue using Facebook as usual.{ref}\u00a0Experiments like this will tell us the short-run effect among experiment participants. This is of course not a silver bullet \u2013 one could raise concerns about how generalizable the estimates are (e.g. Are participants representative of the general population? Do effects remain over time?). No method is perfect, but experiments do address the issue of causality, so they provide critical evidence to understand the link between social media and well-being.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Several recent papers followed this approach. Here I\u2019ll discuss ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "one of them in particular", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", because I find its approach particularly compelling. The analysis relies on a much larger sample than other experiments, and the researchers registered a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.socialscienceregistry.org/trials/3409", "children": [ { "text": "pre-analysis plan", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " to insure themselves against the \u2018analytical flexibility\u2019 criticisms discussed above.{ref}\u00a0The full reference is: Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2019). The welfare effects of social media (No. w25514). National Bureau of Economic Research. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "This experiment was done by four economists: Hunt Allcott, Sarah Eichmeyer, Luca Braghieri and Matthew Gentzkow. They recruited 2753 Facebook users in the US, and randomly selected half of them to stop using Facebook for four weeks. They found that deactivating Facebook led to small but statistically significant improvements in some measures of self-reported well-being.{ref}\u00a0They recruit participants by giving them electronic gift cards. Loosely speaking, the idea was that participants were paid to play a lottery: Everybody received the monetary incentive, but only those who \u2018lost the lottery\u2019 had to deactivate their account.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The chart shows a summary of their estimated effect sizes. As we can see, for all measures the effects are small (amounting to only around a tenth of the standard deviation of the studied variable), and in most cases the effects are actually not statistically significant (the \u2018whiskers\u2019 denoting 95% confidence intervals often include an effect of size zero).{ref}\u00a0The paper provides the following definition for the variables: ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "The happiness variable is the average response to two questions from the Subjective Happiness Scale (Lyubomirsky and Lepper 1999), asking how happy participants were over the past four weeks and how happy they were compared to their peers. Life satisfaction is the sum of responses to three questions from the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al. 1985), such as the level of agreement with the statement, \u201cDuring the past 4 weeks, I was satisfied with my life.\u201d Loneliness is the Three-Item Loneliness Scale (Hughes et al. 2004). Finally, depressed, anxious, absorbed, and bored reflect how much of the time during the past four weeks respondents felt each emotion, using questions from the European Social Survey well-being module (Huppert et al. 2009). The daily text messages allowed us to measure the aspects of subjective well-being that are most important to record in the moment instead of retrospectively. This approach builds on the Experience Sampling Method of Csikszentmihalyi and Larson (2014) and Stone and Shiffman (1994). The variable SMS happiness is the answer to the question, \u201cOverall, how happy do you feel right now on a scale from 1 (not at all happy) to 10 (completely happy)?\u201d The variable SMS positive emotion is an indicator variable for whether the participant reports a positive emotion when asked, \u201cWhat best describes how you felt over the last ten minutes?\u201d, with possible responses such as \u201cangry,\u201d \u201cworried,\u201d \u201cloving/tender,\u201d etc. Finally, SMS not lonely uses the answer to the question, \u201cHow lonely are you feeling right now on a scale from 1 (not at all lonely) to 10 (very lonely)?\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": "{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Allcott and co-authors also compare the treatment effects against the observational correlations in their sample and conclude: \u201c", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "children": [ { "text": "the magnitudes of our causal effects are far smaller than those we would have estimated using the correlational approach of much prior literature\u201d", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-italic" }, { "text": ".{ref}\u00a0To get a sense of the magnitudes of these effects, the authors discuss effect sizes in their original units, focusing on the measures with the largest effects. Happiness is the average response to two questions on a scale from 1 (not a very happy person) to 7 (a very happy person). The control group endline average is 4.47 out of a possible 7, and deactivation caused an average increase of 0.12. This is of course very, very small. In fact, the authors explain that given how small these effect sizes are, they would have been unlikely to have sufficient statistical power to detect any effects if they had used sample sizes similar to those from previous experiments in this field, all of which are much smaller.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The relatively small experimental effect of social media use on subjective well-being has been replicated. ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462", "children": [ { "text": "Another experiment", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " conducted almost at the same time and with a very similar approach, produced similar results.{ref}\u00a0In ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462", "children": [ { "text": "this other experiment", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", quitting Facebook for a week led to a small but statistically significant decrease in self-reported feelings of depression, yet there was no significant effect on other measures of life satisfaction. The full reference is: Mosquera, R., Odunowo, M. M., McNamara, T., Guo, X., & Petrie, R. (2018). The Economic Effects of Facebook. {/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "alt": "", "size": "wide", "type": "image", "filename": "subjective-well-being-1-v2-full.png", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Mechanisms", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In the US, where many of these studies have been conducted, roughly two-thirds of people ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.journalism.org/2018/09/10/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-2018/", "children": [ { "text": "get news from social media", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ", and these platforms have already become a ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/12/10/social-media-outpaces-print-newspapers-in-the-u-s-as-a-news-source/", "children": [ { "text": "more widely accessed source of news than print newspapers", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "I think this link between social media, news consumption and well-being is key.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "In their experiment, Allcott and coauthors found that quitting Facebook did not lead people to use alternative online or offline news sources; so those in the treatment group reported spending less time consuming news overall. This tells us that the effect of social media on well-being is not only relatively small, but also likely mediated by the specific types of content and information that people are exposed to.{ref}This effect has also been replicated: The other contemporary experiment that followed a similar approach (", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462", "children": [ { "text": "Mosquera et al. 2019", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": "), found that individuals facing a Facebook restriction reduced their news consumption.{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The fact that news consumption via social media might be an important factor affecting well-being is not surprising if we consider that news are typically biased towards negative content, and there is ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract", "children": [ { "text": "empirical research", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": " suggesting people are triggered, at a physiological level, when exposed to negative news content.{ref}\u00a0A new paper published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences concludes that all around the world, the average human is more physiologically activated by negative than by positive news stories. The authors come to this conclusion through an experiment: Participants watched 7 randomly ordered BBC World News stories on a laptop computer while wearing noise-cancelling headphones and sensors on their fingers to capture skin conductance and blood volume pulse. The experiment was conducted with 1,156 participants across 17 countries: Brazil, Canada, Chile, China, Denmark, France, Ghana, India, Israel, Italy, Japan, New Zealand, Russia, Senegal, Sweden, the United Kingdom, and the United States. The full reference is Soroka, S., Fournier P., & Nir L., (2019) Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1908369116. Available online ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract", "children": [ { "text": "here", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" }, { "text": ".", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" }, { "text": "{/ref}", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Building and reinforcing a scary overarching narrative around \u201cthe terrible negative effects of social media on well-being\u201d is unhelpful because this fails to recognise that social media is a large and evolving ecosystem where billions of people interact and consume information in many different ways.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "What are the key takeaways?", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The first takeaway is that the association between social media and well-being is complex and reciprocal, which means that simple correlations can be misleading. A careful analysis of survey data reveals that, yes, there is a correlation between social media and well-being; but the relationship works both ways. This becomes clear from the longitudinal studies: Higher use of social media predicts decreases in life satisfaction; and decreasing life satisfaction also predicts subsequent increases in social media use.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "The second takeaway is that the causal effect of social media on well-being is likely small for the average person. The best empirical evidence suggests the impact is much smaller than many news stories suggest and most people believe.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "There is much to be learned about how to make better use of these digital platforms, and there is an important discussion to be had about the opportunity costs of spending a large fraction of our time online. But for this we need to look beyond the sweeping newspaper headlines.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "We need research with more granular data to unpack diverse use patterns, to understand the different effects that certain types of content have on specific population groups. Time alone is a poor metric to gauge effects. As Andrew Przybylski put it: nobody would argue we should study the causes of obesity by investigating \u2018food time\u2019.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "text": "Going forward, the conversation in policy and the news should be much more about strategies to promote positive content and interactions, than about one-size-fits-all restrictions on social media \u2018screen time\u2019.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "spanType": "span-newline" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Data Sources", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 1, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "horizontal-rule", "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28.", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Data Source:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Multiple sources", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": " Description of available measures:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Geographical coverage:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Selected towns and cities", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Link", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": ": ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093", "children": [ { "text": "https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "\u00a0Cigna - Loneliness Report", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Data Source:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " \u00a0Survey of 20,000 Americans", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": " Description of available measures:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Self-reported loneliness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Geographical coverage:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " US", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Link", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": ": ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america", "children": [ { "text": "https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "Office for National Statistics - Loneliness Report", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Data Source:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " \u00a0Community Life Survey", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": " Description of available measures:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Self-reported loneliness", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Geographical coverage:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " UK", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Link", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": ": ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10", "children": [ { "text": "https://www.ons.gov.uk", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "World Value Survey", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Data Source:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " \u00a0Representative national surveys", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": " Description of available measures:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " \u00a0Share of people who interact with friends at least once per month", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Geographical coverage:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Selected countries", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Link", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": ": ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/", "children": [ { "text": "http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "United Nations Database on Household Size and Composition 2018", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Data Source:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Multiple sources", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": " Description of available measures:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Percentage of one-person households as a ratio of total households", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Geographical coverage:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Global, by country", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Link", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": ": ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840", "children": [ { "text": "https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "OECD \u2013 \u00a0How's Life? 2017 Measuring Well-being", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Data Source:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Multiple sources", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": " Description of available measures:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Percentage of people who report that they have friends or relatives whom they can count on in times of trouble", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Geographical coverage:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Global, by country", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Link", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": ": ", "spanType": "span-simple-text" }, { "url": "https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en", "children": [ { "text": "https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "text": [ { "text": "OECD \u2013 Society at a Glance", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "type": "heading", "level": 3, "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "list", "items": [ { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Data Source:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Multiple sources", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": " Description of available measures:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Social media use", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Geographical coverage:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "text": " Global, by country", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "type": "text", "value": [ { "children": [ { "text": "Link:", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-bold" }, { "url": "https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en", "children": [ { "text": "https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en", "spanType": "span-simple-text" } ], "spanType": "span-link" } ], "parseErrors": [] } ], "parseErrors": [] }, { "top": [], "type": "all-charts", "heading": "Interactive charts on loneliness and social connections", "parseErrors": [] } ], "type": "linear-topic-page", "title": "Loneliness and Social Connections", "authors": [ "Esteban Ortiz-Ospina", "Max Roser" ], "excerpt": "Family and friends are important for our well-being. In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections, and review available evidence on the link between social connections and well-being.", "dateline": "February 14, 2020", "subtitle": "Family and friends are important for our well-being. In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections, and review available evidence on the link between social connections and well-being.", "sticky-nav": [], "sidebar-toc": true, "featured-image": "self-reported-loneliness-older-adults-1.png" }, "createdAt": "2020-01-30T17:42:09.000Z", "published": false, "updatedAt": "2023-01-16T21:07:07.000Z", "revisionId": null, "publishedAt": "2020-02-14T05:42:00.000Z", "relatedCharts": [ { "slug": "job-seeker-job-search-pew", "title": "Job search methods", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 0 }, { "slug": "proportion-of-unemployed-using-specific-search-methods", "title": "Job search methods among unemployed jobseekers", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 0 }, { "slug": "people-who-report-having-friends-or-relatives-they-can-count-on", "title": "People who report having friends or relatives they can count on", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 2 }, { "slug": "percentage-of-americans-living-alone-by-age", "title": "Percentage of Americans living alone, by age", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 2 }, { "slug": "adults-living-alone", "title": "Percentage of adults living alone", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 2 }, { "slug": "percentage-of-females-living-alone-in-the-united-states-by-age", "title": "Percentage of females living alone in the United States, by age", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 2 }, { "slug": "percentage-of-males-living-alone-in-the-united-states-by-age", "title": "Percentage of males living alone in the United States, by age", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 2 }, { "slug": "one-person-households", "title": "Percentage of one-person households", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 3 }, { "slug": "labor-productivity-per-hour-pennworldtable", "title": "Productivity: output per hour worked", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 0 }, { "slug": "self-reported-loneliness-older-adults", "title": "Self-reported loneliness among older adults", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 3 }, { "slug": "one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita", "title": "Share of one-person households vs. GDP per capita", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 2 }, { "slug": "time-spent-with-relationships-by-age-us", "title": "Who Americans spend their time with, by age", "variantName": null, "keyChartLevel": 3 } ], "publicationContext": "listed" } |
{ "errors": [ { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag image" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag image" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag image" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag image" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag image" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag image" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag image" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" }, { "name": "unexpected wp component tag", "details": "Found unhandled wp:comment tag list" } ], "numBlocks": 331, "numErrors": 15, "wpTagCounts": { "html": 9, "list": 9, "image": 7, "heading": 57, "paragraph": 243, "owid/summary": 1, "owid/additional-information": 1 }, "htmlTagCounts": { "p": 243, "h2": 4, "h3": 10, "h4": 40, "h5": 3, "ol": 1, "ul": 8, "div": 2, "figure": 11, "iframe": 8 } } |
2020-02-14 05:42:00 | 2024-03-19 18:38:37 | [ "Esteban Ortiz-Ospina" ] |
Family and friends are important for our well-being. In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections, and review available evidence on the link between social connections and well-being. | 2020-01-30 17:42:09 | 2023-01-16 21:07:07 | https://ourworldindata.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/07/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults-1.png | {} |
This article was first published in February 2020. Available research shows that social connections are important for our well-being. Having support from family and friends is important for our happiness and health, and is also instrumental to our ability to share information, learn from others, and seize economic opportunities. In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections across countries and over time, and review available evidence on how and why social connections and loneliness affect our health and emotional welfare, as well as our material well-being. Despite the fact that there is a clear link between social connections and well-being, more research is needed to understand causal mechanisms, effect sizes and changes over time. As we show here, oversimplified narratives that compare loneliness with smoking, or that claim we are living a 'loneliness epidemic', are wrong and unhelpful. **Related articles** [Interpersonal Trust](https://ourworldindata.org/trust) – Trust is an essential part of social connections. Trust is crucial for community, wellbeing, and effective cooperation. [Internet](https://ourworldindata.org/internet) – For many, the internet is now essential for work, finding information, and connecting with others. [Global Health](https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta) – Social connections matter for health. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in global health. [Happiness and Life Satisfaction](https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction) – Social connections matter for our happiness. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in happiness and life satisfaction across societies and locations. **[See all interactive charts on loneliness and social connections ↓](#all-charts)** --- # Social connections --- ## How important are social connections for our health? Dr. Vivek Murthy, former [Surgeon General](https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Surgeon_General_of_the_United_States) of the United States, recently [wrote](https://www.vivekmurthy.com/single-post/2017/10/10/Work-and-the-Loneliness-Epidemic-Harvard-Business-Review): “Loneliness and weak social connections are associated with a reduction in lifespan similar to that caused by smoking 15 cigarettes a day”. This ‘15 cigarettes a day’ figure has been reproduced and reported in the news many times, under headlines such as “Loneliness is as lethal as smoking 15 cigarettes per day”.{ref}There are dozens of articles reporting the ‘15 cigarettes a day’ figure. See for example [this in WebMD](https://www.webmd.com/balance/news/20180504/loneliness-rivals-obesity-smoking-as-health-risk), and [this in The Daily Mail](https://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-5181559/Loneliness-bad-health-smoking-15-day.html). From what I gather, all the articles that cite this figure trace back to Dr. Vivek Murthy and a meta-analysis by Holt-Lunstad et al. (2015), which I discuss in more detail below in this post. When I reviewed this meta-analysis I did find the claim that loneliness is a risk factor comparable to smoking; but I could not find the calculations that lead to the 15 cigarettes benchmark. It’s still unclear to me what the unit of loneliness is and what this translates into in terms of years of lives lost. However, given that 15 cigarettes per day is [about average consumption among smokers](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/consumption-per-smoker-per-day-bounds), it’s possible that the benchmark comes from comparing excess mortality of smoking for the average smoker, vs. excess mortality of loneliness for average levels of loneliness.{/ref} It is indeed quite a shocking comparison since around [7 million deaths](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/smoking-and-secondhand-deaths) globally are attributed to smoking every year, and back-of-the-envelope calculations published in medical journals say [one cigarette reduces your lifespan by 11 minutes](https://www.bmj.com/content/bmj/320/7226/53.1.full.pdf?casa_token=Fcl9ywjt31gAAAAA:9FnQFmC50vZclWEndIVf8Vf4uwlEsKIiQeeQtFXm11_06jJwLcgZn1MS4LPyFsFVnrcCsSSHXnv5). Here we dig deeper to try to understand what the data and research tell us about the link between social relations and health. In a nutshell, my reading of the evidence is as follows: 0. There is a huge amount of evidence showing individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in their life. 1. There is credible theory and explanation of biological mechanisms, whereby isolation can set off unconscious surveillance for social threat, producing cognitive biases, reducing sleep and affecting hormones. 2. It's very likely there is a causal link, but there is no credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect that loneliness has on key metrics of health, such as life expectancy. 3. The fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect of loneliness on health doesn't mean we should dismiss the available evidence. But it does show that more research is needed. ### Observational studies: A first look at the data #### Measuring loneliness Psychologists and social neuroscientists often refer to loneliness as [_painful_ isolation](https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2008-07755-000). The emphasis on painful is there to make a clear distinction between solitude – the state of being alone – and subjective loneliness, which is the distressing feeling that comes from unmet expectations of the types of interpersonal relationships we wish to have. Researchers use several kinds of data to measure solitude and loneliness. The most common source of data are surveys where people are asked about different aspects of their lives, including whether they live alone, how much time they spend with other people in a given window of time (e.g. ‘last week’) or specific context (e.g. ‘at social events, clubs or places of worship’); and whether they experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. ‘I have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with’). Researchers sometimes study these survey responses separately, but often they also aggregate them in a composite index.{ref}The most popular composite index is the [UCLA Loneliness Scale](https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/UCLA_Loneliness_Scale), which was first proposed in 1978, and is based on a 20-item scale designed to measure subjective feelings of loneliness. You can read more about it [here](https://fetzer.org/sites/default/files/images/stories/pdf/selfmeasures/Self_Measures_for_Loneliness_and_Interpersonal_Problems_UCLA_LONELINESS.pdf).{/ref} Surveys [confirm](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0898264312460275) that people respond differently to questions about subjective loneliness and physical social isolation, which suggests people do understand these as two distinct issues. In the chart here I've put together estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness from various sources. The fact that we see such high levels of loneliness, with substantial divergence across countries, explains why this is an important and active research area. Indeed, there are literally hundreds of papers that have used survey data to explore the link between loneliness, solitude, and health. Below is an overview of what these studies find. <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults"/> #### The link between loneliness and physical health Most papers studying the link between loneliness and health find that both objective solitude (e.g. living alone) and subjective loneliness (e.g. frequent self-reported feelings of loneliness) are correlated with higher morbidity (i.e. illness) and higher mortality (i.e. likelihood of death). The relationship between health and loneliness can of course go both ways: lonely people may see their health deteriorate with time; but it may also be the case that people who suffer from poor health end up feeling more lonely later down the line. Because of this two-way relationship it’s important to go beyond cross-sectional correlations and focus on longitudinal studies – these are studies where researchers track the same individuals over time to see if loneliness predicts illness or mortality in the future, after controlling for baseline behaviors and health status. The evidence from longitudinal studies shows that people who experience loneliness during a period of their life tend to be more likely to have worse health later down the line. In the Netherlands, for example, [researchers found](https://academic.oup.com/aje/article-pdf/146/6/510/455063/146-6-510.pdf) that self-reported loneliness among adults aged 55-85 predicted mortality several months later, and this was true after controlling for age, sex, chronic diseases, alcohol use, smoking, self-assessed health condition, and functional limitations.{ref}Penninx, B. W., Van Tilburg, T., Kriegsman, D. M., Deeg, D. J., Boeke, A. J. P., & van Eijk, J. T. M. (1997). Effects of social support and personal coping resources on mortality in older age: The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. American journal of epidemiology, 146(6), 510-519.{/ref} Most studies focus either on subjective loneliness, or on objective isolation. But some studies try to compare both. In a recent meta-analysis covering 70 longitudinal studies, the authors [write](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691614568352):_ “We found no differences between measures of objective and subjective social isolation. Results remain consistent across gender, length of follow-up, and world region.”_ And in the concluding section they highlight that, in their interpretation of the evidence, _“the risk associated with social isolation and loneliness is comparable with well-established risk factors for mortality”_; which include smoking and obesity.{ref}Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on psychological science, 10(2), 227-237.{/ref} #### The link between mental health and subjective well-being In another much-cited review of the evidence, Louise Hawkley and John Cacioppo, two leading experts on this topic, [concluded](https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3874845/) that _“perhaps the most striking finding in this literature is the breadth of emotional and cognitive processes and outcomes that seem susceptible to the influence of loneliness”._{ref}Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of behavioral medicine, 40(2), 218-227. {/ref} Researchers have found that loneliness correlates with subsequent increases in symptoms related to [dementia](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/dementia-death-rates), [depression](https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health#depression), and many other issues related to [mental health](https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health); and this holds after controlling for demographic variables, objective social isolation, stress, and baseline levels of cognitive function. There is also research that suggests a link between loneliness and lower [happiness](https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction), and we discuss this in more detail [here](http://www.ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-friends). ### Mechanisms Experiments with social animals, like rats, [show](https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/106/52/22393.full.pdf) that induced isolation can lead to a higher risk of death from cancer. Humans and rats are of course very different; but experts, such as Hawkley and Cacioppo, argue that these experiments are important because they tell us something meaningful about a shared biological mechanism. In a review of the evidence, Susan Pinker writes: “If our big brains evolved to interact, loneliness would be an early warning system—a built-in alarm that sent a biological signal to members who had somehow become separated from the group”.{ref}Pinker, S. (2015). The village effect: How face-to-face contact can make us healthier and happier. Vintage Canada.{/ref} Indeed, there’s evidence of social regulation of gene expression in humans: [studies suggest](https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/108/7/3080.full.pdf) perceived loneliness can switch on/off genes that regulate our immune systems, and it is this what then affects the health of humans, or other animals that evolved with similar defence mechanisms.{ref}Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). Transcript origin analysis identifies antigen-presenting cells as primary targets of socially regulated gene expression in leukocytes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 3080-3085.{/ref} ### Causality and implications The bulk of evidence from observational studies and biological mechanisms, described above, implies that loneliness most likely matters for our health and well being. But do we really know how much it matters relative to other important risk factors? The key point here is that estimates are likely biased to some extent. The findings from longitudinal studies that track individuals over time are insightful, but they cannot rule out that the relationship might be partly driven by other factors that we cannot observe. Even the studies linking loneliness and genetics can be subject to this [omitted-variable bias](https://mru.org/dictionary-economics/omitted-variable-economics), because a genetic predisposition to loneliness may drive both loneliness and health outcomes.{ref}Two concrete papers that show this is a real concern are: - Abdellaoui, A., Sanchez-Roige, S., Sealock, J., Treur, J. L., Dennis, J., Fontanillas, P., … & Baselmans, B. (2018). Phenome-wide investigation of health outcomes associated with genetic predisposition to loneliness. bioRxiv, 468835. -Abdellaoui, A., Chen, H. Y., Willemsen, G., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Verweij, K. J., … & Cacioppo, J. T. (2019). Associations between loneliness and personality are mostly driven by a genetic association with neuroticism. Journal of personality, 87(2), 386-397.{/ref} I could not find credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect.{ref}If you know of relevant studies that do provide such estimates, please let me know at [email protected].{/ref} But the fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect doesn't mean we should dismiss the available evidence. On the contrary – it would be great if we had evidence from randomized control trials that test positive interventions to reduce loneliness, to understand better if the ‘15 cigarettes per day’ comparison from the Surgeon General of the US is roughly correct, at least for the average person. Having a better understanding of the magnitude of the effect is important, not only because loneliness is common, but also because it’s complex and unequally experienced by people around the world. As the chart above shows, there are large differences in self-reported loneliness across countries. We should understand how important these differences are for the distribution of health and well-being. ## Are we happier when we spend more time with others? In 1938, a group of Harvard researchers decided to start a research program to track the lives of a group of young men, in what eventually became one of the longest and most famous longitudinal studies of its kind. The idea was to track the development of a group of teenage boys through periodic interviews and medical checkups, with the aim of understanding how their health and well-being evolved as they grew up.{ref}For historical context: The Harvard Graduate School of Education – the first to admit women – opened admissions to women in 1920; and the Harvard Medical School accepted its first female enrollees in 1945, even though the first woman applied in 1847. You can read more about the hard-earned gains for women at Harvard here: [https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/](https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/){/ref} Today, more than 80 years later, it is one of the longest running research programmes in social science. It is called the [Harvard Study of Adult Development](https://www.adultdevelopmentstudy.org/) and it is still running. The program started with 724 boys, and researchers continue to monitor today the health and well-being of those initial participants who are still alive, most in their late 90s.{ref}The original participants for this study came from two groups: a cohort of 268 men, 19 years of age, from the Harvard classes of 1939-1944; and a group of 456 caucasian men, ages 11-16, from underprivileged neighborhoods of Boston. Over the years the project evolved, enrolling more participants, and many of the children of the original participants are included in the study today.{/ref} This is a unique scientific exercise – most longitudinal studies do not last this long because too many participants drop out, researchers move on to other projects (or even die), or the funding dries up. So what have we learned from this unique study? Robert Waldinger, the current director of the study, summarized – in what is now one of the [most viewed TED Talks to date](https://www.ted.com/talks/robert_waldinger_what_makes_a_good_life_lessons_from_the_longest_study_on_happiness?language=en) – the findings from decades of research. The main result, he concluded, is that social connections are one of the most important factors for people’s [happiness](https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction) and [health](https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta). He said: “Those who kept warm relationships got to live longer and happier, and the loners often died earlier.” Here I take a closer look at the evidence and show more research that finds a consistent link between social connections and happiness. But before we get to the details, let me explain why I think this link is important. As most people can attest from personal experience, striving for happiness is not easy. In fact, the search for happiness can become a source of _unhappiness_ – there are studies that show actively pursuing happiness [can end up decreasing it](https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf). The data shows that [income and happiness are clearly related](https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#the-link-between-happiness-and-income); but we also know from surveys that people often [overestimate the impact of income on happiness](https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/spc3.12386). Social relations might be the missing link: In rich countries, where minimum material living conditions are [often satisfied](https://ourworldindata.org/extreme-poverty#historical-poverty-in-today-s-rich-countries), people may struggle to become happier because they are targeting material rather than social goals. ### The cross-sectional correlation between happiness and friends The [World Values Survey (WVS)](http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/wvs.jsp) is a large cross-country research project that collects data from a series of representative national surveys. In the fourth wave (1999-2004) the WVS asked respondents hundreds of questions about their lives, including whether they were part of social or religious groups, how often they spent time with friends, and whether they felt happy with their life. By comparing self-reported happiness among those with and without frequent social interactions, we can get an idea of whether there is indeed a raw correlation between happiness and social relations across different societies. In the chart I show this comparison: The green points correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends at least once per month; while the blue dots correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends less often.{ref}The question on frequency of interaction with friends was asked in the context of time spent at work, clubs or places of worship. The options included ‘weekly’, ‘once or twice a month’, ‘only a few times a month’, and ‘not at all’. The options for happiness, on the other hand, included ‘very happy’, ‘quite happy’, ‘not very happy’ and ‘not at all happy’. In the chart I took this data, and I split the sample in two: I classified respondents as ‘often meeting with friends’ if they said they met with friends once a month or more frequently; and I classified them as ‘rarely meeting with friends’ if they said they met with friends ‘once a year’ or ‘not at all’. Then, for each of these two groups, I plotted the share who reported being ‘very happy’ or 'quite happy’.{/ref} This chart shows that in almost all countries, people who often spend time with their friends report to be happier than those who spend less time with friends.{ref}The estimates have of course a margin of error that varies with the survey size. The difference between the green and blue dots is statistically significant for about half of the countries (i.e. for about half of the countries in this chart, the 95% confidence intervals around the blue and green dots would not overlap). {/ref} <Image filename="happiness-and-friends-v3-e1564090552891.png" alt=""/> ### The link between social relations and happiness over time The chart above gives a cross-sectional perspective – it’s just a snapshot that compares different people at a given point in time. What happens if we look at changes in social relations and happiness over time? There is a large academic literature in medicine and psychology that shows individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in life (you can read more about this in my [post on social relations and health](http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being)); and similarly, there are also many studies that show that changes in social relations predict changes in happiness and life satisfaction. One of the research papers that draws on data from the famous Harvard Study of Adult Development, for example, looked at the experiences of 82 married participants and their spouses, and found that greater self-reported couple attachment predicted lower levels of depression and greater life satisfaction 2.5 years later.{ref}Waldinger, R. J., Cohen, S., Schulz, M. S., & Crowell, J. A. (2015). Security of attachment to spouses in late life: Concurrent and prospective links with cognitive and emotional well-being. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 516-529. Available online [here](https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4579537/).{/ref} Other studies with larger population samples have also found a similar cross-temporal link: perceived social isolation predicts subsequent changes in depressive symptoms but not vice versa, and this holds after controlling for demographic variables and stress.{ref}See for example: - Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and aging, 25(2), 453. - Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and aging, 21(1), 140.{/ref} Searching for happiness is typically an intentional and active pursuit. Is it the case that people tend to become happier when they purposely decide to improve their social relations? This is a tough empirical question to test; but [a recent study](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0956797618761660) found evidence pointing in this direction. Using a large representative survey in Germany, researchers asked participants to report, in text format, ideas for how they could improve their life satisfaction. Based on these answers, the researchers then investigated which types of ideas predicted changes in life satisfaction one year later. The researchers found that those who reported socially-engaged strategies (e.g. “I plan to spend more time with friends and family”) often reported improvements in life satisfaction one year later; while those who described other non-social active pursuits (e.g. “I plan to find a better job”) did not report increased life satisfaction.{ref}The researchers used an open question: “What could you do to ensure that you will be more satisfied in the future?”. There are of course many types of answers that people can provide to such an open-ended question, so the authors decided to classify each individual answer into one of two categories, depending on whether or not the respondents described a ‘socially engaged strategy’. This process of classifying answers could naturally raise questions: the authors could manipulate the subjective classification of answers to fit their favourite hypothesis. In order to insure themselves against this criticism, the researchers relied on three independent coders, and they stipulated the classification method and the expected results in a [pre-analysis plan](https://blogs.worldbank.org/impactevaluations/a-pre-analysis-plan-checklist). You find more details in the paper: Rohrer, J. M., Richter, D., Brümmer, M., Wagner, G. G., & Schmukle, S. C. (2018). Successfully striving for happiness: Socially engaged pursuits predict increases in life satisfaction. Psychological science, 29(8), 1291-1298.{/ref} ### Are we happier when we spend more time with others? From decades of research we know that social relations predict mental well-being over time; and from a recent study we also know that people who actively decide to improve their social relations often report becoming happier. So yes, people are happier when they spend more time with friends. Does this mean that if we have an exogenous shock to our social relations this will have a permanent negative effect on our happiness? We can’t really answer this with the available evidence. More research is needed to really understand the causal mechanisms that drive the link between happiness and social relations.{ref}The key point to keep in mind is that we are not talking about experimental evidence. People who frequently interact with friends, or people who say they want to engage more in ‘social strategies to improve their lives’ are likely different; and although the researchers do try to control for this, it is very hard to measure all the relevant characteristics that might help drive life satisfaction. Similarly, asking people about strategies to improve their lives is difficult, and although the qualitative strategy from the paper above is interesting, it’s still hard to draw accurate comparisons: it’s possible that ‘non-social’ strategies, for example, are more difficult or stressful to pursue; so part of the lower apparent impact that these strategies have on happiness might come from the fact that some people are trying but failing to find a better job.{/ref} Despite this, however, I think that we should take the available observational evidence seriously. In a way, the causal impact from a random shock is less interesting than the evidence from active strategies that people might take to improve their happiness. People who get divorced, for example, often report a short-term drop in life satisfaction; but over time they tend to recover and eventually end up being more satisfied with their life than shortly before they divorced (you can read more about this in our [entry on happiness and life satisfaction](https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#life-satisfaction-through-life-events)). It makes sense to consider the possibility that healthy social relationships are a key missing piece for human wellbeing. Among other things, this would help explain the paradoxical result from studies where actively pursuing happiness [apparently ends up decreasing it](https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf). ## The importance of social networks for innovation and productivity Economies today are increasingly built on ideas. Consider, for example, commercial aviation. After a flight, _Southwest Airlines _can unload and reload passengers to get the same airliner back in the air in about 10 minutes – this is half the time the same process took in the 1970s. As the economist John Cochrane [explains](https://review.chicagobooth.edu/economics/2019/article/what-success-rock-climbing-tells-us-about-economic-growth), this achievement is as much about ideas and know-how as it is about technology. _Southwest Airlines_, and many other carriers after them, have been able to expand their output because they now have [more efficient protocols](https://www.npr.org/2015/06/28/418147961/the-man-who-saved-southwest-airlines-with-a-10-minute-idea) for ‘turning planes’ between flights. In 2018 Paul Romer won the Nobel Prize in Economics “for integrating technological innovations into long-run macroeconomic analysis”. Romer first fleshed out this observation in his [seminal 1990 paper](http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf), titled “Endogenous Technological Change”. At the heart of his contributions was precisely this observation: the diffusion of ideas fosters economic and productivity growth.{ref}Romer’s seminal paper, with nearly 30,000 citations in Google Scholar is Romer, P. M. (1990). Endogenous technological change. Journal of Political Economy, 98 (5, Part 2), S71-S102. Available online [here](http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf). After this paper he wrote many other related articles elaborating on the same ideas. See, for example, Romer, P. M. (1992). Two strategies for economic development: using ideas and producing ideas. The World Bank Economic Review, 6(suppl_1), 63-91. Available online [here](https://academic.oup.com/wber/article-abstract/6/suppl_1/63/1656015?redirectedFrom=fulltext).{/ref} Romer’s theory of “endogenous growth” – placing ideas at the heart of economic technologies – made economists ask entirely new questions about the drivers of growth. Where do ideas come from? How do ideas spread? Economists have asked these questions of the data, and their findings suggest social networks play a key role. Friendship and professional ties matter for economic growth because people tend to develop new ideas by interacting and learning from others who are close to them. In a nutshell, social networks facilitate the diffusion of ideas across individuals and firms, and because of this, they play an important role in productivity growth. Here’s how this works. ### Knowledge spillovers, social networks, and patent citations Economic theory suggests we should expect social connections to have an effect on productivity via “knowledge spillovers". The basic idea is that social connections make it easier for knowledge to travel farther and faster. Knowledge is different from many physical assets, in that it can be used by many people at the same time; so the diffusion of knowledge creates productivity gains that spillover through society. As Thomas Jefferson once said: _“He who receives an idea from me, receives instruction himself without lessening mine; as he who lights his taper at mine, receives light without darkening me”._{ref}Letter to Isaac McPherson. August 13, 1813. Online [here](https://en.wikisource.org/wiki/Letter_to_Isaac_McPherson_-_August_13,_1813).{/ref} What is the empirical value of this theory? What do ‘knowledge spillovers’ actually look like in the real world? Do scientists and engineers really rely on social connections to disseminate their new ideas and inventions? To answer these questions economists have looked into historical data from [patents](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patent-applications-per-million). Patents are government licences used to register the intellectual property of new inventions, ranging [from machines to medical drugs](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patents-usa-by-category). When a patent application is filed, inventors are required to include a list of citations, in which they describe how their technological development builds upon the results of earlier patents. These historical records of patent citations give us an overview of how knowledge flows – patent citations are the ‘paper trail’ left by ideas as they travel, evolve, and make new ideas possible. One of the first analyses of patent citations in the context of knowledge spillovers was done by Adam Jaffe, Manuel Trajtenberg and Rebecca Henderson, in [a much-cited paper](https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813) published in the Quarterly Journal of Economics in 1993.{ref}Jaffe, A. B., Trajtenberg, M., & Henderson, R. (1993). Geographic localization of knowledge spillovers as evidenced by patent citations. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 108(3), 577-598. Available online [here](https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813).{/ref} Jaffe and coauthors studied, in particular, the geographic distribution of citation patterns in the US. If social connections did not matter for new ideas and growth, then we should expect that new inventions are just as likely to cite patents from far away as they are to cite the work of their neighbours. In contrast, Jaffe and coauthors found that new patents were disproportionately more likely to cite previous _local_ patents. In other words, they found that patent citations were strongly geographically concentrated. According to their estimates, citations were three to four times as likely to come from the same state as the originating patent – this is consistent with the idea that social connections and proximity do indeed matter for innovation. After this initial study, several other papers have used patent citations to explore the determinants of knowledge spillovers, with a broader perspective on ‘proximity’. Ajay Agrawal, Devesh Kapur and John McHale used the ethnicity of researchers as a proxy for social connectedness, and [found](http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.515.3876&rep=rep1&type=pdf) that co-location and co-ethnicity both increase the probability of knowledge flow between inventors. Co-location increases the probability of a knowledge flow by 24% (assuming non-co-ethnic inventors) and co-ethnicity increases the probability by 14% (assuming non-co-located inventors). Again, these large concentration patterns seem consistent with the idea that proximity and social networks matter for innovation.{ref} Agrawal, A., Kapur, D., & McHale, J. (2008). How do spatial and social proximity influence knowledge flows? Evidence from patent data. Journal of Urban Economics, 64(2), 258-269.{/ref} ### Disentangling geographic proximity and social interactions The correlation between proximity and patent citations partly captures the effect that local factors may have on innovation (e.g. geography, regulation or infrastructure around industry hubs where similar workers and firms concentrate). However, the evidence shows that social connections matter for the transmission of knowledge even after we control for these other factors. In [a paper](https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259) published in 2018 in the _Journal of Economic Perspectives_, Michael Bailey, Rachel Cao, Theresa Kuchler, Johannes Stroebel, and Arlene Wong combined patent records with Facebook data from the US, and found that social connectedness between US counties (measured by the frequency of Facebook ties between counties) predicts the probability of cross-county patent citations. This is true, even after accounting for the patents’ technology class and geographic distance between counties.{ref} The full reference of the paper is: Bailey, M., Cao, R., Kuchler, T., Stroebel, J., & Wong, A. (2018). Social connectedness: measurement, determinants, and effects. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32(3), 259-80. Available online [here](https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259).{/ref} This means that the probability that an inventor builds on the work of another inventor in a different part of the country can be partly explained by how well-connected their social backgrounds are, even after controlling for geographic distance and other demographic factors.{ref}To be more precise, Bailey and co-authors find that the effect of doubling social connectedness on the probability of citation remains significant and large, at 4.9 percent, even after controlling for geographic distance. When they add controls for cross-country differences on socioeconomic indicators (income, education, ethnicity, etc.) they actually find that the estimated relationship between social connectedness and patent citation increases somewhat as a result of these further controls. This finding suggests that the relationship between geographic distance and the probability of patent citation, viewed in isolation, may be partially capturing effects of information flows associated with social connectedness.{/ref} The implications of this result are of great consequence in the age of the internet. The development of new communication-enabling technologies, such as [the rise of social media](http://www.ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media), have opened new possibilities for creating and maintaining social connections, and this is likely to have had a positive effect on the transmission of knowledge. ### Social connections and (material) well-being Globally, the total economic output per person today is [4.4-times larger](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&tab=chart&year=1950&time=1870..latest&country=OWID_WRL) than in 1950 (even after accounting for inflation). This was achieved over a period during which people tended towards working [fewer hours](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/annual-working-hours-per-worker?tab=chart&stackMode=absolute&time=1950..2017&country=BRA~COL~FRA~DEU~NLD~GBR~USA®ion=World). We now produce more whilst working less. As our interactive chart here shows, this historical achievement is underpinned by productivity growth: in many countries, workers produce much higher output per hour today than in 1950. This is partly because of new machines and technologies, but also because we have new shared knowledge about how to work more efficiently. The research on social connections and innovation suggests that one important way to improve material living standards is to invest in digital communication technologies. This lowers the costs of creating and maintaining personal and professional ties, which facilitates the diffusion of ideas and knowledge, creating positive productivity spillovers. <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/labor-productivity-per-hour-PennWorldTable?time=1950..2017&country=BRA+DEU+IND+JPN+KOR+GBR+USA"/> ## Additional information Social networks are diverse and complex, so talking about “social connectedness” is actually a simplification that deserves some discussion. Researchers studying social networks have shown that the flow of information and knowledge through people is not only a function of “connectedness” as measured by the number of links people have, but also a function of the specific structures of the networks (e.g. the extent to which social ties are tightly clustered). For the diffusion of information, the concept of ‘homophily’, for example, is key. Homophily is the tendency of agents to associate with other agents who have similar characteristics. This manifests itself in many ways – for example, in the fact that friendship ties are typically strongly related to ethnicity. If one is interested in how information spreads through social learning, patterns such as homophily or social segregation are important to understand. For example, in the labor market, homophily isolates workers of one ethnicity from workers of another ethnicity, which then limits the extent to which individuals in one group hear about openings and opportunities known to the other group. Social networks research in this area shows that, in general, as homophily increases, the propensity for a diffusion to gain hold within a particular group rises, sometimes at the expense of the speed and extent of diffusion throughout the entire population. Similarly, diversity may play a role. Observational studies [show](https://science.sciencemag.org/content/328/5981/1029) that the diversity of individuals’ relationships is strongly correlated with the economic development of communities. You can read more about this in Jackson, M. O., Rogers, B. W., & Zenou, Y. (2017). The economic consequences of social-network structure. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 49-95. Available online [here](https://www.econstor.eu/bitstream/10419/175053/1/Wp1116.pdf). ## The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, West German families that had relatives in East Germany saw an increase in their income, and this growth in incomes was [significantly higher](https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf) than that of comparable West German families without such family ties to the East. Why did this happen? Are personal and family ties really so important to our economic outcomes that they result in systematic differences in income growth? Researchers have studied this question and have found empirical support for the idea that social connections do, indeed, have large effects on economic outcomes. ### A natural experiment: Friends and incomes after the fall of the Berlin Wall Measuring the impact of personal relations on economic outcomes is difficult. This is because these two aspects of our life are often closely intertwined – social ties facilitate economic interactions, but at the same time people with whom we interact for economic reasons end up becoming acquaintances or even friends. To overcome this difficulty, the economists Konrad Burchardi and Tarek Hassan studied a historical event that suddenly opened up new economic interactions, where previously only non-economic social connections were possible: The fall of the Berlin Wall.{ref}Konrad B. Burchardi and Tarek A. Hassan. "The economic impact of social ties: Evidence from German reunification." The Quarterly Journal of Economics 128.3 (2013): 1219-1271. Available online [here](https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf).{/ref} For decades private economic exchange between East and West Germany was virtually impossible, but at the same time people on both sides maintained social ties for non-economic reasons. After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, trade and collaboration between the two Germanies suddenly became feasible. Burchardi and Hassan studied the impact that this sudden change had on household incomes. After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989 there was [growth in incomes](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&time=1500..2018&country=~DEU) across Germany as a whole; but the interesting finding from Burchardi and Hassan is that income growth for households in West Germany who had ties to at least one relative in the East was much higher – six percentage points higher – than that of comparable households without such ties. Burchardi and Hassan argue that West German households that had ties to East Germany had a comparative advantage in seizing the new economic opportunities in the East. Having personal relationships with East Germans gave them access to valuable economic information – information regarding local demand conditions, and about the quality of East German assets that were offered to investors. These gains from social connections actually added up at the regional level. West German regions that had a higher concentration of households with social ties to the East, enjoyed substantially higher growth in incomes in the early 1990s. A one standard deviation rise in the share of households with social ties to East Germany in 1989 was associated with a 4.6 percentage point rise in income per capita over six years. ### Personal relations and job search: Evidence from Europe The unique situation of the German reunification shows that social ties that are initially unrelated to economic interests can become important for economic success. The key mechanism is that personal connections allow us to seize new economic opportunities. This may sound abstract, but in fact most of us have experienced this in our own lives. Perhaps the best example is job searching: personal contacts and connections are one of the most common ways for people to find a job. In the book _The Human Network_, the economist Matthew Jackson draws on decades of research to help explain how social connections affect power, beliefs and behaviors. With regard to job networks and social capital, he writes: “_If you have ever struggled to find a job, you are not alone. Without well-connected friends or family it is hard to make it in any industry [...]. Finding a job without having some personal connection to someone already employed at a firm is more the exception than the rule.” _{ref}Pages 239-241 in Jackson, M. O. (2019). _The Human Network: How Your Social Position Determines Your Power, Beliefs, and Behaviors_. Pantheon.{/ref} Our interactive chart here shows evidence for Jackson’s claim using data from European countries. The chart shows the distribution of job search methods by country. Data for Spain is shown by default, but you can explore data for different countries using the option ‘Change country’. In Spain, 84% of the unemployed rely on contacts – friends, relatives, trade unions – to find a job. It is the most common job-search method. Direct applications and ads rank below, on places two and three. If you plot the same data for other countries you’ll see that personal contacts are a very important job-search method everywhere in Europe, but there is some variation across countries. At the lowest end of the spectrum, in Belgium, about 38% of people rely on contacts to find a job; at the other end, in Greece and Latvia, 90% of job seekers rely on this method. _(NB. _[_This interactive map_](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-friends-relatives-trade-unions-in-job-search?tab=map)_ shows the same data, but focusing specifically on the proportion of job seekers relying on contacts for job search across Europe.)_ <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-specific-search-methods"/> ### Friends and relatives remain a popular method for finding a job, even in the age of digital job search platforms The chart showing job search methods across Europe relies on data from the European Labor Force Survey, 2006-2008. We have not found more recent data that allows cross-country comparisons, but surveys from individual high-income countries show that this remains relevant today, even after the expansion of online job search engines, and [the rise of social media platforms](https://ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media). In the US, data from the Pew Research Center shows that the internet is the most important resource for job search, but 66% of survey respondents still say they relied on connections with close friends or family in their most recent search for a job. These two methods are of course not mutually exclusive, and the boundaries between ‘internet’ and ‘personal contact’ are becoming less clear as job search platforms become more integrated with social media. <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/job-seeker-job-search-pew"/> ### Personal relations matter for trade Personal relations can also provide information and social collateral that can be valuable for economic interactions across national borders. The political scientist Anna Lee Saxenian [analyzed](https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf) the biographies of Chinese and Indian engineers who migrated to California in the 1970s. She found that as these skilled immigrants excelled in their personal careers, they started leveraging their social ties to relatives and friends in their home regions. This allowed them to outsource important operations for a large fraction of Silicon Valley firms.{ref}Saxenian, A. (2000). Silicon Valley’s new immigrant entrepreneurs. UC San DiegoWorking Papers. The Center for Comparative Immigration Studies. Available online [here](https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf).{/ref} Saxenian argues that by connecting Silicon Valley firms to low-cost, high-quality labor in India and China, they became instrumental for the success of Silicon Valley and crucial in the emergence of their home regions as major hubs of the global IT services industry. ### Personal relations matter for _collective_ economic outcomes In some situations personal connections may facilitate economic transactions for one person (or group of people) at the expense of another one. This is what many people have in mind when they think about job referrals. If there is only one job vacancy, giving the job to a friend means not giving the job to someone else. If social connections become a vehicle for favouritism, this can lead to inefficiencies. In many contexts, however, the opposite is true. Social connections often have a net-positive effect on aggregate economic outcomes because much of the impact of social connections on economic transactions is not zero-sum – for example, when social connections reduce information frictions and allow people to learn from each other. As we discuss in a [companion post](http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-networks-innovation-and-productivity), there is empirical evidence showing these knowledge spillovers are substantial. ### Social networks can affect economic outcomes via agglomeration effects The concentration of firms and workers in ‘industry hubs’ is partly explained by the fact that social connections create economic externalities: Agglomeration leads to denser social connections, and this has positive spillover effects on productivity. Again the fall of the Berlin Wall offers clear evidence of this. When Berlin was split, economic activity in West Berlin shifted away from the historic business district, known as ‘Mitte’. As a consequence property prices there fell relative to the new economic areas that emerged further away from the Wall. When the Wall came down the distribution of economic activity within the city changed quickly once again: The old business district began to re-emerge and economic activity shifted back to the historic center. This is the conclusion of a careful econometric study, published by Gabriel Ahlfeldt, Stephen Redding, Daniel Sturm and Nikolaus Wolf in the journal _Econometrica_.{ref} Ahlfeldt, G. M., Redding, S. J., Sturm, D. M., & Wolf, N. (2015). The economics of density: Evidence from the Berlin Wall. Econometrica, 83(6), 2127-2189. Online [here](https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.3982/ECTA10876).{/ref} The three maps show the evolution of land prices in Berlin. These maps, from Ahlfeldt and co-authors, illustrate graphically the impact that the Wall had on the geography of economic activity. The vertical scale measures relative land prices; this means higher elevation, highlighted with darker shades, denotes higher prices.{ref} The authors use the same vertical scale for each figure, and land prices are normalized to have a mean of 1 in each year, so the levels of the land price surfaces in each map are comparable.{/ref} The map on the left shows that in 1936 there are two areas with very high land values in West Berlin and both are close to the center (they are both part of a concentric ring around the pre-war central business district, located in the middle). In 1986, we see smaller peak values; in fact, we see that the pre-war peak closest to the Wall is entirely eliminated following division, as this area ceased to be an important center of commercial and retail activity. Finally, in 2006, we see again higher peaks; and we see that the area closest to the Wall is reemerging as a center of high land values. This evolution is a great example of the economics of density: The areas in West Berlin close to the old central business district became less productive during the division but regained their productivity and attractiveness again soon after reunification. Knowledge, public resources, and opportunities for trade and collaboration that had been inaccessible for decades, suddenly became available after the fall of the Wall.{ref} Ahlfeldt and coauthors disentangle agglomeration effects from other forces that took place at the same time. In particular, they account for the fact that the Berlin Wall had an impact both on agglomeration forces (i.e. “production externalities” determined by the surrounding density of workers); as well as on "production fundamentals" (i.e. the physical characteristics of the different neighborhoods where workers and firms concentrate).{/ref} <Image filename="berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015.png" alt=""/> ### The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes Social connections matter hugely for economic outcomes. Personal relations, even those that we maintain for non-economic reasons, often give us access to information and provide us with social collateral for economic transactions – from buying a house to getting a job. This can have positive consequences that extend beyond the individuals directly involved in the transactions. When personal and professional ties facilitate the exchange of information and knowledge, this can bring benefits to society as a whole. The implication from this is that social connections are not only important because they [affect our emotional well-being](https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being), but also because, as we show here, they affect our _material_ well-being too. --- # Loneliness, solitude and social isolation --- ## Living alone is becoming increasingly common around the world In the US, the share of adults who live alone nearly doubled over the last 50 years. This is not only happening in the US: single-person households have become increasingly common in many countries across the world, from Angola to Japan. Historical records show that this ‘rise of living alone’ started in early-industrialized countries over a century ago, accelerating around 1950. In countries such as Norway and Sweden, single-person households were rare a century ago, but today they account for nearly half of all households. In some cities they are already the majority. Surveys and census data from recent decades show that people are more likely to live alone in rich countries, and the prevalence of single-person households is unprecedented historically. Social connections – including contact with friends and family – are [important](https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being) for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as single-person households become more common, there will be new challenges to connect and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where welfare states are weaker. But it’s important to keep things in perspective. It’s unhelpful to compare the rise of living alone with a ‘loneliness epidemic’, which is what newspaper articles often write in [alarming headlines](https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA). Loneliness and solitude are [not the same](http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone), and the evidence suggests that self-reported loneliness has [_not_ been growing](http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic) in recent decades. ### Historical perspective on people living alone: Evidence from rich countries Historical records of inhabitants across villages and cities in today’s rich countries give us insights into how uncommon it was for people to live alone in the past. The chart here, adapted from [a paper](https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true) by the historian Keith Snell, shows estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data. Each dot corresponds to an estimate for one settlement in Europe, North America, Japan or Britain.{ref}Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28. Online [here](https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true).{/ref} The share of one-person households remained fairly steady between the early modern period and through the 19th century – typically below 10%. Then growth started in the twentieth century, accelerating in the 1960s. The current prevalence of one-person households is unprecedented historically. The highest point recorded in this chart corresponds to Stockholm, in 2012, where 60% of households consist of one person. <Image filename="historical-one-person-households.png" alt=""/> ### The rise of one-person households across the world For recent decades, census data can be combined with data from large cross-country surveys, to provide a global perspective on the proportion of households with only one member (i.e. the proportion of single-person households). This gives us a proxy for the prevalence of solitary living arrangements.{ref}The percentage of single-person households is a proxy for ‘solitary living’. However, it’s important to emphasise that these two things are not really the same. Single-person households include those where a person lives alone in an individual housing unit, but they also include people who live independently as lodgers in a separate room within a larger housing unit with other occupant. So technically, there are some people who live in a ‘single-person household’, but they don’t really live alone.{/ref} We produced this chart combining individual reports from statistical country offices, cross-country surveys such as the [_Demographic and Health Surveys_](https://dhsprogram.com/), and estimates published in the EU’s _[Eurostat](https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat)_, the UN’s [Demographic Year Books](https://unstats.un.org/unsd/demographic-social/products/dyb/), and the [Deutschland in Daten](https://figshare.com/articles/German_Time_Series_Dataset_1834_2012/1450809/1) dataset. The chart shows that the trend of rising single-person households extends across all world regions. There are large differences between countries – from more than 40% in northern European countries to 1% in low-income Asian countries. _(NB. For the US and Canada there are long-run time series from census data that let us directly track the share of people who live alone. This is shown in _[_this other chart_](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/adults-living-alone)_, where you can see the same trend.) _[](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households) <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households"/> ### Living arrangements and prosperity National income per capita and the share of one-person households are strongly correlated: As the chart here shows, people are more likely to live alone in rich countries. In this interactive chart you can move the slider to see changes over time. This reveals that the rise of single-person households tends to be larger in countries where GDP per capita has grown more. _(NB. You can also see the correlation over time in _[_this other_](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?stackMode=relative&time=1990..2016)_ scatter plot comparing average growth in GDP vs average growth in one-person households)._ These correlations are partly due to the fact that people who can afford to, often choose to live alone. Indeed, rising incomes in many countries are likely part of the reason why people are more likely to live alone today than in the past. But there must be more to it since even at the same level of incomes there are clear differences between regions. In particular, Asian countries have systematically fewer one-person households than African countries with comparable GDP levels. Ghana and Pakistan, for example, have similar GDP per capita, but in Pakistan one-person households are extremely rare, while in Ghana they are common (about 1 in 4). This suggests culture and country-specific factors also play an important role. Additionally, there are other non-cultural country-specific factors that are likely to play a role. In particular, rich countries often have more extensive social support networks, so people in these countries find it easier to take risks. Living alone is more risky in poorer countries, because there’s often less supply of services and infrastructure to support more solitary living arrangements. And finally, it’s also likely that some of the causality runs in the opposite direction. It’s not only that incomes, culture or welfare states enable people to live alone, but also that for many workers attaining higher incomes in today’s economy often demands changes in living arrangements. Migration[ from rural to urban areas](https://ourworldindata.org/urbanization) is the prime example. <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita"/> ### Is the rise of one-person households a problem? Social connections – including contact with friends and family – are [important](https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being) for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as the ‘rise of living alone’ continues, there will be new challenges to connect people and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where communication technologies are [less developed](https://ourworldindata.org/internet) and welfare states are [weaker](https://ourworldindata.org/government-spending#high-income-countries-spend-more-on-social-protection-than-low-income-countries). But, it’s also important to keep in mind that living alone is [not the same](http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone) as feeling lonely. There’s evidence that living alone is, by itself, a poor predictor of loneliness. Self-reported loneliness has [not been growing ](http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic)in recent decades, and in fact, the countries where people are most likely to say they have support from family and friends, are the same countries – in Scandinavia – where a large fraction of the population lives alone. Incomes and freedom of choice are not the only drivers of the ‘rise of living alone’; but it would be remiss to ignore they do contribute to this trend. [Higher incomes](https://ourworldindata.org/economic-growth), [economic transitions](https://ourworldindata.org/structural-transformation-and-deindustrialization-evidence-from-todays-rich-countries) that enable migration from agriculture in rural areas into manufacturing and services in cities, and [rising female participation in labor markets](https://ourworldindata.org/female-labor-supply) all play a role. People are more likely to live alone today than in the past partly because they are increasingly able to do so. ## Loneliness and aloneness are not the same There is a popular [perception](https://www.vice.com/sv/article/mvn7eb/the-swedish-theory-of-love-or-why-we-swedes-are-so-lonely-234) that countries in Northern Europe are heavily individualistic and because of this people in these societies tend to be much lonelier. The data, however, does not support this claim. What is true is that in countries such as Denmark and Switzerland, it is [very common](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE) for people to live alone. But contrary to what many believe, this does not translate into higher levels of self-reported loneliness. Below we discuss the data in more detail, and show that this can be partly explained by the fact that loneliness and aloneness are just not the same. Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it’s important not to conflate them. ### Self-reported loneliness Loneliness describes a _subjective_ feeling; this is conceptually distinct from _objective_ physical isolation. In the chart here we show estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness among older adults. The data comes from various surveys asking people directly whether they often experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. _“I have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with”_). The differences in the prevalence of loneliness across countries are very large. At the bottom of the list, Denmark, Switzerland, Sweden and the US all have rates below 30%, while at the top of the list Greece, Israel and Italy all have rates of close to or above 50%. The data is available only for fifteen countries, but this sample does not suggest that ['rich individualistic societies'](https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Individualistic_culture) are lonelier than others. In fact, at the very bottom are two countries in which the share of people living alone is among the [highest](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE) in the world.[](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults) <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults"/> ### Perceptions of social support Similar to loneliness, we can measure perceptions of social support by asking people directly. This is what the polling organization _Gallup_ did in their flagship [_World Poll_](https://www.gallup.com/analytics/232838/world-poll.aspx) survey. Specifically, they asked: _“If you were in trouble, do you have relatives or friends you can count on to help you whenever you need them, or not?”_ The chart here presents the results from this survey, plotting the share of people who responded “yes” to this question.{ref} The data for this chart comes from a table published in the appendix of the OECD report “How's Life? 2017 Measuring Well-Being”, available online [here](https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en).{/ref} Differences across countries are not very large. The lowest and highest average levels of support corresponds to Mexico and Iceland, at 80% and 98% respectively. The second point that stands out is that, again, there’s no support for the claim that richer countries that are considered to be more individualistic (e.g. North European countries) have lower levels of family and friendship support. <Chart url="https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/people-who-report-having-friends-or-relatives-they-can-count-on"/> ### Distinguishing between aloneness and loneliness Are people who live alone more likely to say they feel lonely? The researchers Caitlin Coyle and Elizabeth Dugan explored this question using data from the_ Health and Retirement Study_ in the US. In their [analysis](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275), covering almost 12,000 respondents over the years 2006-2008, they found that loneliness and social isolation are not highly correlated at the individual level.{ref}Coyle, C. E., & Dugan, E. (2012). Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. Journal of aging and health, 24(8), 1346-1363. Online [here](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275).{/ref} To be precise, Coyle and Dugan found a correlation coefficient of 0.2. That means the correlation is positive, and it is statistically larger than zero; but it’s not very large in absolute terms. Their study does not provide the raw data, but in general, a scatter plot with correlation 0.2 is pretty much just a [cloud of dots](https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Correlation_and_dependence). This means you cannot predict much of the variability in loneliness from observed differences in living arrangements. Some people who live alone are lonely, but many are not. Concluding that someone who lives alone must feel lonely is very often wrong.{ref}There’s similar evidence from other countries. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the _Community Life Survey_, in which they ask about living arrangements and loneliness. This survey finds that while the reported frequency of loneliness is certainly higher among those who live alone, the correlation is not higher that of other ‘risk factors’ such as disability, age and marital status. You find more information in the ONS report [here](https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10).{/ref} ### Being alone and feeling lonely are not the same People do not seem to be more lonely in societies that are [traditionally labeled](https://www.hofstede-insights.com/country-comparison/finland,norway,sweden/) as 'individualistic'. What is true is that in these societies it is [particularly common](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE) for people to live alone. But being alone and feeling lonely don’t always go hand in hand. Many people feel lonely even if they are not physically isolated; and many people who are physically isolated do not feel lonely. The aggregate statistics confirm this. Surveys that ask people about living arrangements, time use, and feelings of loneliness, find that solitude, by itself, does not predict feelings of loneliness. The fact that people in individualistic countries are not more likely to feel lonely may of course capture differences in expectations regarding social relations. But to the extent that loneliness is a subjective experience, these cross-country differences are still important to understand well-being. Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it’s important not to conflate them. ## Changes in loneliness over time: Is there a loneliness epidemic? The media seems to have agreed that rich countries are experiencing a ‘loneliness epidemic’. There are literally [thousands](https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA) of newspaper articles that use this exact expression. What is the evidence for this? The word ‘epidemic’ suggests that things are getting much worse and loneliness is increasing rapidly. But does the data in fact show that societies are becoming lonelier? Despite the popularity of the claim, there is surprisingly no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates. It is [true](http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone) that more people are living alone around the world. But loneliness and aloneness are not the same. As we explain in a [companion post](http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone), spending time alone is not a good predictor of whether people feel lonely, or have weaker social support. As we explain later, today’s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today’s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than older adults in the past. Surveys covering older adults in other rich countries, including Finland, Germany, England and Sweden, point in the same direction – it’s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations in these countries.{ref} See the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online [here](https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable).{/ref} Social connections – including contact with friends and family – are important for our [health and emotional welfare](https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being), as well as for our [material well-being](https://ourworldindata.org/personal-relations-econ-outcomes). Loneliness is indeed an important problem, but it’s crucial to have a nuanced conversation based on facts. The headlines that claim we are witnessing a ‘loneliness epidemic’ are wrong and unhelpful. ### Are young people more lonely than older adults? One statistic that is often used to argue that loneliness is increasing, is that young people today are lonelier than older adults. This begs two questions: (i) Is it true that younger people are lonelier, and (ii) does this show that loneliness is increasing? Let’s start with the first question. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the [Community Life Survey](https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/community-life-survey-2018-19), in which they ask people how often they feel lonely. In the bar chart here we show a breakdown of self-reported loneliness by age group. According to this data, those aged 16 to 24 are the group most likely to report feeling lonely, with 10% feeling lonely “often or always”. In contrast, those aged 65 years and older are the group least likely to report feeling lonely, with 3% feeling lonely “often or always”. Many people tend to associate loneliness with older age, so this pattern might seem surprising. But surveys from several other rich countries have found the same. In [New Zealand](http://archive.stats.govt.nz/browse_for_stats/people_and_communities/older_people/loneliness-in-nz-2010-NZGSS/loneliness-in-nz.aspx), [Japan](http://files.kff.org/attachment/Report-Loneliness-and-Social-Isolation-in-the-United-States-the-United-Kingdom-and-Japan-An-International-Survey) and the [US](https://www.multivu.com/players/English/8294451-cigna-us-loneliness-survey/docs/IndexReport_1524069371598-173525450.pdf), young adults also report feeling lonely more often than older adults. So, yes, in rich countries we find that younger people are more likely to report feeling lonely.{ref}The emphasis on ‘rich countries’ is important. A study exploring self-reported loneliness across nine former Soviet countries, found that older adults report higher levels of loneliness. This shows it's wrong to generalize from the available data, which is primarily from rich countries. For more details see: Stickley, A., Koyanagi, A., Roberts, B., Richardson, E., Abbott, P., Tumanov, S., & McKee, M. (2013). Loneliness: its correlates and association with health behaviours and outcomes in nine countries of the former Soviet Union. PloS one, 8(7), e67978. Online [here](https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable).{/ref} What about the second question? Does this mean that loneliness is increasing? Here, the answer is ‘no’. Cross-sectional comparisons are not informative about changes _over time_, because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle. To be able to say something meaningful about changes in loneliness over time, we need to distinguish between changes for _individuals_ over time (do people become lonelier as they get older?) and changes across generations (are people of the same age lonelier today than in the past?). Let’s dig deeper and explore these questions separately. <Image filename="Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England.png" alt=""/> ### Do we become lonelier as we get older? To understand how loneliness changes across our life cycle, we need loneliness data from surveys that track the same individuals over time, up until old age. In a [study](https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download) published in the journal _Psychology and Aging_, Louise Hawkley and co-authors examine two such surveys, with data for adults older than 50 years in the US.{ref} Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online [here](https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download).{/ref} They found that after age 50 – which is the earliest age of participants in their study – loneliness tended to decrease, until about 75, after which it began to increase again. The authors explain in their paper that the increase in loneliness after 75 was explained by a decline in health and the loss of a spouse or partner. When adjusting for these factors, they found that loneliness continued declining into ‘oldest old age’. This shows that there are two forces at play. On the one hand, there seems to be a direct relationship between age and loneliness, whereby loneliness _decreases_ with age as our social expectations adapt, and we become [more selective](https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1207/S15324834BASP2304_2?casa_token=885gubc-kPAAAAAA:gP8oVDQ-085Ftuh1-pvmG4Ms4-O58KDka4hjgK5Nq7RjS3ZsESymhhPo_owFbaf5MufXSx2LvFc0oA) about relating with contacts who bring positive emotions. On the other hand, there seems to be an indirect association pushing in the opposite direction, whereby loneliness _increases_ with age, because our health deteriorates and we lose relatives and friends. In our middle age the direct effect dominates, but once we enter advanced old age, the negative indirect effect starts dominating. This complex relationship between age and loneliness shows why comparing old and young people at a given point in time is misleading. Cross-sectional comparisons are just not informative about the evolution of loneliness over time, because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle. ### Loneliness trends over time: Are people lonelier today than in the past? In the ‘loneliness epidemic’ narrative, it is often implied that if we compare two individuals of the same age – one today and another one a generation ago – we would find that the one today is more likely to feel lonely. This is based on the idea that there have been societal changes – such as the [rise of living alone](http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone) – that make newer generations more likely to feel lonely. In their [study](https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download), Louise Hawkley and co-authors searched for evidence of these ‘cohort trends’ in the US, but didn’t find any. There was very little difference in self-reported loneliness of people born in different generations. Those that were born in 1920-1947 experienced the same changes of loneliness throughout their lives as those born in 1948-1965. It’s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations. An [article](https://unherd.com/2019/05/is-the-epidemic-of-loneliness-fake-news/) by Tom Chivers explains this result from the study very well: _“[Hawkley and co-authors] found that “newer” old people (baby boomers born 1948-1965) are no more likely to think of themselves as lonely than “older” old people (born 1920-1947), and that older people have not become more likely to think of themselves as lonely in the last decade (2005 – 2016)... The average older person appears to be no more likely to be lonely than they were a decade ago.”_ There are studies with data from other rich countries pointing in the same direction. In Sweden, repeated cross-sectional surveys with adults aged 85, 90, and 95 years old, found no increase in loneliness over a ten-year interval.{ref} Nyqvist, F., Cattan, M., Conradsson, M., Näsman, M., & Gustafsson, Y. (2017). Prevalence of loneliness over ten years among the oldest old. Scandinavian journal of public health, 45(4), 411-418. Available online [here](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1403494817697511). For similar results in other countries, including Finland, Germany and England, see the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online [here](https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable).{/ref} These studies find no evidence of cohort effects across older adults. What about evidence for adolescents? In the aptly titled [paper](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789) “Rethinking ‘Generation Me’: A Study of Cohort Effects from 1976-2006”, the psychologists Kali Trzesniewski and Brent Donnellan used repeated cohort surveys to explore whether successive groups of high-school graduates were becoming more lonely in the US. They also found no evidence of cohort trends. Newer generations of high-school seniors were not more likely to report feelings of loneliness than earlier generations.{ref} Trzesniewski and Donnellan relied on data from the [Monitoring the Future Project](http://www.monitoringthefuture.org), an ongoing survey of young Americans. Their analysis extends beyond loneliness. The paper abstract explains the results as follows: _“Social commentators have argued that changes over the last decades have coalesced to create a relatively unique generation of young people. However, using large samples of U.S. high-school seniors from 1976 to 2006 (Total N = 477,380), we found little evidence of meaningful change in egotism, self-enhancement, individualism, self-esteem, locus of control, hopelessness, happiness, life satisfaction, loneliness, antisocial behavior, time spent working or watching television, political activity, the importance of religion, and the importance of social status over the last 30 years.” _The full reference of the paper is: Trzesniewski, K. H., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Rethinking “Generation Me”: A study of cohort effects from 1976-2006. Perspectives on psychological science, 5(1), 58-75. Available online [here](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789).{/ref} The psychologists Matthew Clark, Natalie Loxton, and Stephanie Tobin [replicated](https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0146167214557007) this analysis, using the same survey, but focusing on all age groups, not only high-school seniors. The chart here shows their results. They found no signs of increasing loneliness across all age groups. In fact, they found a very small but statistically significant _decline in loneliness _for high school students in the US.{ref} To measure loneliness, the project used a six-item questionnaire, where participants used a 1 (disagree) to 5 (agree) scale to respond to the following statements: ‘‘_A lot of times I feel lonely_,’’ ‘‘_There is always someone I can turn to if I need help (reverse scored)_,’’ ‘‘_I often feel left out of things_,’’ ‘‘_There is usually someone I can talk to if I need to (reverse scored)_,’’ ‘‘_I often wish I had more good friends_,’’ and ‘_‘I usually have a few good friends around that I can get together with (reverse scored)._’’ The project began collecting loneliness data for Grade 12 students in 1977, and data for Grades 8 and 10 in 1991. {/ref} _(NB. The vertical axis in this chart is truncated, following the presentation in the original paper. The truncated axis is helpful to highlight the trend; but the takeaway is that the changes in levels are extremely small, so the trend is effectively flat in absolute terms, even if the slope is statistically different from zero)._ <Image filename="declining-loneliness-of-US-students.png" alt=""/> ### Loneliness deserves attention, but headlines that claim we are witnessing a ‘loneliness epidemic’ are not true, and are actually unhelpful The magazine _The Economist _wrote [an article](https://www.economist.com/international/2018/09/01/loneliness-is-a-serious-public-health-problem) in 2018 with the title “Loneliness is a serious public-health problem”. In one key paragraph, the article reads: “Historical data about loneliness are scant. But isolation does seem to be increasing, so loneliness may be too.” The data we have found shows that this reasoning is actually incorrect. Surveys from rich countries do not suggest there has been an increase in loneliness over time. Today’s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today’s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than did adults of their age in the past. That’s of course not to say we should not pay attention to these topics. It’s important to provide support to people who [suffer from loneliness](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults), just as it is important to pay attention to the policy challenges that come from large societal changes such as the [rise of living alone](http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone). However, inaccurate, over-simplified narratives are unhelpful to really understand these complex challenges. There is an epidemic of headlines that claim we are experiencing a “loneliness epidemic”, but there is no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates. --- # Social media and social networks --- ## The rise of social media platforms ## Are Facebook and other social media platforms bad for our well-being? Facebook, Youtube, Whatsapp, WeChat, and Instagram are the top five social media platforms globally, with over [one billion active users each](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/users-by-social-media-platform). In most rich countries the proportion of young people using online social networks [exceeds 90%](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/percentage-of-young-people-engaging-in-social-networking-online) and teens spend on average [more than 4 hours online every day](https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/daily-time-spent-on-the-internet-by-young-people). We’re repeatedly told in the news that social media is bad for us. The stories are often alarming, suggesting social media and smartphones are responsible for sweeping negative trends, [from rising suicide rates in the US](https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression), to [widespread loss in memory, and reduced sleep and attention spans](https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-families/social-media-mental-health-negative-effects-depression-anxiety-addiction-memory-a8307196.html). These worrying headlines often go together with implicit or explicit recommendations to limit the amount of time we spend on social media. Indeed, smartphones today come with built-in “screen time” apps that let us track and limit how much time we spend online. At the same time, most of us would agree that digital social media platforms can make our lives easier in many ways – opening doors to new information, connecting us with people who are far away, and helping us to be more flexible with work. What does the research tell us about the causal impact of social media use on our well-being? In a nutshell: From my reading of the scientific literature, I do not believe that the available evidence today supports the sweeping newspaper headlines. Yes, there is evidence suggesting a causal negative effect, but the size of these causal effects is heterogeneous and much, much smaller than the news headlines suggest. There are still plenty of good reasons to reflect on the impact of social media in society, and there is much we can all learn to make better use of these complex digital platforms. But this requires going beyond universal claims. Let’s take a look at the evidence. ### Comparisons across individuals Most of the news stories that claim social media has a negative impact on well-being rely on data from surveys comparing individuals with different levels of social media use as evidence. In the chart, I show one concrete example of this type of correlational analysis. The chart plots the average amount of time that people spend on social media each day, among people who are and aren’t happy with the amount of time spent on these platforms. The data comes from an app called [Moment](https://inthemoment.io/), which tracks the amount of time users spend on social media platforms on their smartphones. The app also asks people a yes/no question: “Are you happy with your time spent?” As we can see there is quite a bit of heterogeneity across platforms, but the pattern is clear: People who say they are happy with how much time they spend on social media spend less time on these platforms. Or put differently, using social media more heavily is correlated with less satisfaction. This is certainly interesting, but we should be careful not to jump to conclusions – the correlation actually raises as many questions as it answers.{ref}A concrete example of how this correlational evidence is reported in the news can be found in [this article from The Economist](https://www.economist.com/graphic-detail/2018/05/18/how-heavy-use-of-social-media-is-linked-to-mental-illness), where a very similar chart is presented under the headline “How heavy use of social media is linked to mental illness”.{/ref} <Image filename="TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-–-happy-vs-unhappy-users.png" alt=""/> Does this pattern hold if we control for user characteristics like age and gender? Would we get similar results if we focused on other measures of well-being beyond ‘happy with time spent’? The answer to both questions is ‘no’. Depending on what outcome variables you focus on, and depending on which demographic characteristics you account for, you will get a different result. It is therefore not surprising that some empirical academic studies have found negative correlations; while others actually report positive correlations.{ref}Here are some examples of studies reporting a positive correlation between social media use and subjective well-being: - Kim, J. and Lee, J. (2011). The facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number of face- book friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. CyberPsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(6):359–364 - Gonzales, A. L. and Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my facebook wall: Effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(1-2):79–83. - Valenzuela, S., Park, N., and Kee, K. F. (2009). Is there social capital in a social network site?: Facebook use and college students’ life satisfaction, trust, and participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14(4):875–901.{/ref} Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski [published a paper](https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-018-0506-1) earlier this year in the journal _Nature_ where they illustrated that given the flexibility to analyze the data (i.e. given the number of possible choices researchers have when it comes to processing and interpreting the vast data from these large surveys), scientists [could have written thousands of papers](https://socialsciences.nature.com/users/200472-amy-orben/posts/42763-beyond-cherry-picking) describing positive, negative and non-significant associations. Different ways of measuring well-being and social media use will yield different results, even for the same population.{ref} It’s also important to mention that the correlations between social media use and wellbeing are not generally comparable across countries and time. For example, commentators[ often highlight](https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression) that depression and suicide rates among adolescents in the US have been going up ever since the rise of smartphone, and that this is a clear red flag. Yet as Andrew Przybylski explained [in a recent interview](http://rationallyspeakingpodcast.org/show/rs-237-andy-przybylski-on-is-screen-time-bad-for-you.html), this is just not a general observation: _“You don't see things like it in more tech-saturated countries, or in other industrialized countries. You don't see, two or three years ahead of the United States, the South Koreans and the Japanese having spiking rates of self-harm or depression”_.{/ref} Even the answers to some of the most fundamental questions are unclear: Do we actually know in which direction the relationship might be going? Does frequent social media use translate into lower happiness, or is it the other way around – are anxious, stressed or depressed people particularly prone to use social media? This takes us to another branch of the literature: longitudinal studies that track individuals over time to measure changes in social media use and well-being. ### Studies of social media use and well-being over time One longitudinal study that has received much attention on this subject was [published](https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143) by Holly Shakya and Nicholas Christakis in the _American Journal of Epidemiology_ in 2017. It used data from a survey that tracked a group of 5,208 Americans over the period 2013 - 2015, and found an increase in Facebook activity was associated with a future _decrease_ in reported mental health.{ref}The full reference is: Shakya, H. B., & Christakis, N. A. (2017). Association of Facebook use with compromised well-being: A longitudinal study. _American journal of epidemiology_, _185_(3), 203-211. Online [here](https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143).{/ref} Two years later, Amy Orben, Tobias Dienlin and Andrew Przybylski [published a paper](https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf) in the _Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences_ using a similar source of data. They relied on a longitudinal survey from the UK covering 12,672 teenagers over the period 2009 - 2016, and reached a different conclusion. They found that there was a small and reciprocal relationship: social media use predicted small decreases in life satisfaction; but it was also the case that decreasing life satisfaction predicted subsequent increases in social media use.{ref}The full reference is: Orben, A., Dienlin, T., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(21), 10226-10228. Online [here](https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf).{/ref} Summarizing their research in _The Guardian_, Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski [explained](https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2019/jul/07/too-much-screen-time-hurts-kids-where-is-evidence): _“we did find some small trends over time – these were mostly clustered in data provided by teenage girls… But – and this is key – it’s not an exaggeration to say that these effects were minuscule by the standards of science and trivial if you want to inform personal parenting decisions. Our results indicated that 99.6% of the variability in adolescent girls’ satisfaction with life had nothing to do with how much they used social media.”_ In their paper Orben and co-authors argue again that these large datasets allow many different types of empirical tests; so it is [natural to expect conflicting results across studies](https://slatestarcodex.com/2014/12/12/beware-the-man-of-one-study/), particularly if there is noise in measurement and the true effect sizes are small.{ref}What’s more, if the cutoff for a statistically significant finding is 5% and you test 100 independent hypotheses of which in reality none are true, you will (in expectation) still find 5 significant associations in either direction simply by chance. Here is a good example and discussion of how this works: [https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1](https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1).{/ref} Orben and co-authors tested thousands of empirical tests and indeed, some of these tests could have been interpreted on their own as evidence of a strong negative effect for social media – but clearly the broader picture is important. When looking at the results from all their thousands of tests, they concluded that social media effects were nuanced, small at best and reciprocal over time.{ref}One more piece of evidence that confirms this conclusion, comes from a more recent long-run study that tracked 500 adolescents for eight consecutive years, between the ages of 13 and 20, and found that _ "increased time spent on social media was not associated with increased mental health issues across development."_ The full reference is Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L., & Booth, M. (2019). Does time spent using social media impact mental health?: An eight year longitudinal study. _Computers in Human Behavior_, 106160. Available online [here](https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0747563219303723).{/ref} ### Facebook experiments Establishing causal impacts through observational studies that track the well-being of individuals over time is difficult. First, there are measurement issues. Long-run surveys that track people are expensive and impose a high burden on participants, so they do not allow in-depth high-frequency data collection, and instead focus on broad trends across a wide range of topics. Orben and co-authors, for example, rely on the [_Understanding Society Survey_](https://www.understandingsociety.ac.uk/) from the UK, which covers a wide range of themes such as family life, education, employment, finance, health and wellbeing. Specifically on social media use, this survey only ask how many hours teenagers remember using apps during normal weekdays, which is of course an informative but noisy measure of actual use (a fact that Orben and co-authors mention in their paper). Second, there are limitations from unobservable variables. Frequent users of social media are likely different from less frequent users in ways that are hard to measure – no matter how many questions you include in a survey, there will always be relevant factors you cannot account for in the analysis. Given these limitations, an obvious alternative is to run an experiment: you can, for example, offer people money to stop using Facebook for a while and then check the effect by comparing these “treated participants” against a control group that is allowed to continue using Facebook as usual.{ref} Experiments like this will tell us the short-run effect among experiment participants. This is of course not a silver bullet – one could raise concerns about how generalizable the estimates are (e.g. Are participants representative of the general population? Do effects remain over time?). No method is perfect, but experiments do address the issue of causality, so they provide critical evidence to understand the link between social media and well-being.{/ref} Several recent papers followed this approach. Here I’ll discuss [one of them in particular](https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf), because I find its approach particularly compelling. The analysis relies on a much larger sample than other experiments, and the researchers registered a [pre-analysis plan](https://www.socialscienceregistry.org/trials/3409) to insure themselves against the ‘analytical flexibility’ criticisms discussed above.{ref} The full reference is: Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2019). The welfare effects of social media (No. w25514). National Bureau of Economic Research. Available online [here](https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf).{/ref} This experiment was done by four economists: Hunt Allcott, Sarah Eichmeyer, Luca Braghieri and Matthew Gentzkow. They recruited 2753 Facebook users in the US, and randomly selected half of them to stop using Facebook for four weeks. They found that deactivating Facebook led to small but statistically significant improvements in some measures of self-reported well-being.{ref} They recruit participants by giving them electronic gift cards. Loosely speaking, the idea was that participants were paid to play a lottery: Everybody received the monetary incentive, but only those who ‘lost the lottery’ had to deactivate their account.{/ref} The chart shows a summary of their estimated effect sizes. As we can see, for all measures the effects are small (amounting to only around a tenth of the standard deviation of the studied variable), and in most cases the effects are actually not statistically significant (the ‘whiskers’ denoting 95% confidence intervals often include an effect of size zero).{ref} The paper provides the following definition for the variables: _The happiness variable is the average response to two questions from the Subjective Happiness Scale (Lyubomirsky and Lepper 1999), asking how happy participants were over the past four weeks and how happy they were compared to their peers. Life satisfaction is the sum of responses to three questions from the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al. 1985), such as the level of agreement with the statement, “During the past 4 weeks, I was satisfied with my life.” Loneliness is the Three-Item Loneliness Scale (Hughes et al. 2004). Finally, depressed, anxious, absorbed, and bored reflect how much of the time during the past four weeks respondents felt each emotion, using questions from the European Social Survey well-being module (Huppert et al. 2009). The daily text messages allowed us to measure the aspects of subjective well-being that are most important to record in the moment instead of retrospectively. This approach builds on the Experience Sampling Method of Csikszentmihalyi and Larson (2014) and Stone and Shiffman (1994). The variable SMS happiness is the answer to the question, “Overall, how happy do you feel right now on a scale from 1 (not at all happy) to 10 (completely happy)?” The variable SMS positive emotion is an indicator variable for whether the participant reports a positive emotion when asked, “What best describes how you felt over the last ten minutes?”, with possible responses such as “angry,” “worried,” “loving/tender,” etc. Finally, SMS not lonely uses the answer to the question, “How lonely are you feeling right now on a scale from 1 (not at all lonely) to 10 (very lonely)?”_{/ref} Allcott and co-authors also compare the treatment effects against the observational correlations in their sample and conclude: “_the magnitudes of our causal effects are far smaller than those we would have estimated using the correlational approach of much prior literature”_.{ref} To get a sense of the magnitudes of these effects, the authors discuss effect sizes in their original units, focusing on the measures with the largest effects. Happiness is the average response to two questions on a scale from 1 (not a very happy person) to 7 (a very happy person). The control group endline average is 4.47 out of a possible 7, and deactivation caused an average increase of 0.12. This is of course very, very small. In fact, the authors explain that given how small these effect sizes are, they would have been unlikely to have sufficient statistical power to detect any effects if they had used sample sizes similar to those from previous experiments in this field, all of which are much smaller.{/ref} The relatively small experimental effect of social media use on subjective well-being has been replicated. [Another experiment](https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462) conducted almost at the same time and with a very similar approach, produced similar results.{ref} In [this other experiment](https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462), quitting Facebook for a week led to a small but statistically significant decrease in self-reported feelings of depression, yet there was no significant effect on other measures of life satisfaction. The full reference is: Mosquera, R., Odunowo, M. M., McNamara, T., Guo, X., & Petrie, R. (2018). The Economic Effects of Facebook. {/ref} <Image filename="subjective-well-being-1-v2-full.png" alt=""/> ### Mechanisms In the US, where many of these studies have been conducted, roughly two-thirds of people [get news from social media](https://www.journalism.org/2018/09/10/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-2018/), and these platforms have already become a [more widely accessed source of news than print newspapers](https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/12/10/social-media-outpaces-print-newspapers-in-the-u-s-as-a-news-source/). I think this link between social media, news consumption and well-being is key. In their experiment, Allcott and coauthors found that quitting Facebook did not lead people to use alternative online or offline news sources; so those in the treatment group reported spending less time consuming news overall. This tells us that the effect of social media on well-being is not only relatively small, but also likely mediated by the specific types of content and information that people are exposed to.{ref}This effect has also been replicated: The other contemporary experiment that followed a similar approach ([Mosquera et al. 2019](https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462)), found that individuals facing a Facebook restriction reduced their news consumption.{/ref} The fact that news consumption via social media might be an important factor affecting well-being is not surprising if we consider that news are typically biased towards negative content, and there is [empirical research](https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract) suggesting people are triggered, at a physiological level, when exposed to negative news content.{ref} A new paper published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences concludes that all around the world, the average human is more physiologically activated by negative than by positive news stories. The authors come to this conclusion through an experiment: Participants watched 7 randomly ordered BBC World News stories on a laptop computer while wearing noise-cancelling headphones and sensors on their fingers to capture skin conductance and blood volume pulse. The experiment was conducted with 1,156 participants across 17 countries: Brazil, Canada, Chile, China, Denmark, France, Ghana, India, Israel, Italy, Japan, New Zealand, Russia, Senegal, Sweden, the United Kingdom, and the United States. The full reference is Soroka, S., Fournier P., & Nir L., (2019) Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1908369116. Available online [here](https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract). {/ref} Building and reinforcing a scary overarching narrative around “the terrible negative effects of social media on well-being” is unhelpful because this fails to recognise that social media is a large and evolving ecosystem where billions of people interact and consume information in many different ways. ### What are the key takeaways? The first takeaway is that the association between social media and well-being is complex and reciprocal, which means that simple correlations can be misleading. A careful analysis of survey data reveals that, yes, there is a correlation between social media and well-being; but the relationship works both ways. This becomes clear from the longitudinal studies: Higher use of social media predicts decreases in life satisfaction; and decreasing life satisfaction also predicts subsequent increases in social media use. The second takeaway is that the causal effect of social media on well-being is likely small for the average person. The best empirical evidence suggests the impact is much smaller than many news stories suggest and most people believe. There is much to be learned about how to make better use of these digital platforms, and there is an important discussion to be had about the opportunity costs of spending a large fraction of our time online. But for this we need to look beyond the sweeping newspaper headlines. We need research with more granular data to unpack diverse use patterns, to understand the different effects that certain types of content have on specific population groups. Time alone is a poor metric to gauge effects. As Andrew Przybylski put it: nobody would argue we should study the causes of obesity by investigating ‘food time’. Going forward, the conversation in policy and the news should be much more about strategies to promote positive content and interactions, than about one-size-fits-all restrictions on social media ‘screen time’. --- # Data Sources --- ### Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28. * **Data Source:** Multiple sources * ** Description of available measures:** Estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data * **Geographical coverage:** Selected towns and cities * **Link**: [https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093](https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093) ### Cigna - Loneliness Report * **Data Source:** Survey of 20,000 Americans * ** Description of available measures:** Self-reported loneliness * **Geographical coverage:** US * **Link**: [https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america](https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america) ### Office for National Statistics - Loneliness Report * **Data Source:** Community Life Survey * ** Description of available measures:** Self-reported loneliness * **Geographical coverage:** UK * **Link**: [https://www.ons.gov.uk](https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10) ### World Value Survey * **Data Source:** Representative national surveys * ** Description of available measures:** Share of people who interact with friends at least once per month * **Geographical coverage:** Selected countries * **Link**: [http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/](http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/) ### United Nations Database on Household Size and Composition 2018 * **Data Source:** Multiple sources * ** Description of available measures:** Percentage of one-person households as a ratio of total households * **Geographical coverage:** Global, by country * **Link**: [https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840](https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840) ### OECD – How's Life? 2017 Measuring Well-being * **Data Source:** Multiple sources * ** Description of available measures:** Percentage of people who report that they have friends or relatives whom they can count on in times of trouble * **Geographical coverage:** Global, by country * **Link**: [https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en](https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en) ### OECD – Society at a Glance * **Data Source:** Multiple sources * ** Description of available measures:** Social media use * **Geographical coverage:** Global, by country * **Link:**[https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en](https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en) <AllCharts heading="Interactive charts on loneliness and social connections"/> | { "id": 29766, "date": "2020-02-14T05:42:00", "guid": { "rendered": "https://owid.cloud/?page_id=29766" }, "link": "https://owid.cloud/social-connections-and-loneliness", "meta": { "owid_publication_context_meta_field": [], "owid_key_performance_indicators_meta_field": { "raw": "Loneliness is common across the world.", "rendered": "<p>Loneliness is common across the world.</p>\n" } }, "slug": "social-connections-and-loneliness", "tags": [ 196, 197, 198, 195 ], "type": "page", "title": { "rendered": "Loneliness and Social Connections" }, "_links": { "self": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/pages/29766" } ], "about": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/types/page" } ], "author": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/users/10", "embeddable": true } ], "curies": [ { "href": "https://api.w.org/{rel}", "name": "wp", "templated": true } ], "replies": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/comments?post=29766", "embeddable": true } ], "wp:term": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/categories?post=29766", "taxonomy": "category", "embeddable": true }, { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/tags?post=29766", "taxonomy": "post_tag", "embeddable": true } ], "collection": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/pages" } ], "wp:attachment": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/media?parent=29766" } ], "version-history": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/pages/29766/revisions", "count": 25 } ], "wp:featuredmedia": [ { "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/media/23957", "embeddable": true } ], "predecessor-version": [ { "id": 32170, "href": "https://owid.cloud/wp-json/wp/v2/pages/29766/revisions/32170" } ] }, "author": 10, "parent": 0, "status": "publish", "content": { "rendered": "\n<div class=\"blog-info\">This article was first published in February 2020.</div>\n\n\n\n<p>Available research shows that social connections are important for our well-being. Having support from family and friends is important for our happiness and health, and is also instrumental to our ability to share information, learn from others, and seize economic opportunities.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections across countries and over time, and review available evidence on how and why social connections and loneliness affect our health and emotional welfare, as well as our material well-being.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Despite the fact that there is a clear link between social connections and well-being, more research is needed to understand causal mechanisms, effect sizes and changes over time. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>As we show here, oversimplified narratives that compare loneliness with smoking, or that claim we are living a ‘loneliness epidemic’, are wrong and unhelpful. </p>\n\n\n\t<div class=\"wp-block-owid-summary\">\n\t\t<h2>Summary</h2>\n\t\t\n\n<ul><li><a href=\"http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#how-important-are-social-connections-for-our-health\">Social connections matter directly for our happiness and health.</a></li><li><a href=\"http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#the-importance-of-personal-relations-for-economic-outcomes\">Social connections are instrumental to prosperity and material well-being.</a></li><li><a href=\"http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#causality-and-implications\">Despite the clear link between social connections and well-being, more research is needed to understand causal mechanisms and effect sizes. In particular, more research is needed to pin down the effect that loneliness has on life expectancy.</a></li><li><a href=\"http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#loneliness-solitude-and-social-isolation\">Solitary living arrangements are becoming increasingly common around the world, but there is no evidence that self-reported loneliness is increasing over time. This can be explained by the fact that loneliness and aloneness are not the same.</a></li><li><a href=\"http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#the-rise-of-social-media-platforms\">Social media platforms have transformed social connections around the world. </a></li><li><a href=\"http://ourworldindata.org/social-connections-and-loneliness/#are-facebook-and-other-social-media-platforms-bad-for-our-well-being\">The causal effect that social media has on adolescent well-being is likely negative, but it is also likely heterogeneous across the population, and much smaller than newspaper headlines suggest.</a></li></ul>\n\n\n\t</div>\n\n\n<p><strong>Related articles</strong></p>\n\n\n\n<p><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/trust\">Interpersonal Trust</a> \u2013 Trust is an essential part of social connections. Trust is crucial for community, wellbeing, and effective cooperation.</p>\n\n\n\n<p><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/internet\">Internet</a> \u2013 For many, the internet is now essential for work, finding information, and connecting with others.</p>\n\n\n\n<p><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta\">Global Health</a> \u2013 Social connections matter for health. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in global health.</p>\n\n\n\n<p><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction\">Happiness and Life Satisfaction</a> \u2013 Social connections matter for our happiness. In this article we provide an overview of key trends in happiness and life satisfaction across societies and locations.</p>\n\n\n\n<h2>Social connections</h2>\n\n\n\n<h3>How important are social connections for our health?</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>Dr. Vivek Murthy, former <a href=\"https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Surgeon_General_of_the_United_States\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">Surgeon General</a> of the United States, recently <a href=\"https://www.vivekmurthy.com/single-post/2017/10/10/Work-and-the-Loneliness-Epidemic-Harvard-Business-Review\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">wrote</a>: \u201cLoneliness and weak social connections are associated with a reduction in lifespan similar to that caused by smoking 15 cigarettes a day\u201d.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This \u201815 cigarettes a day\u2019 figure has been reproduced and reported in the news many times, under headlines such as \u201cLoneliness is as lethal as smoking 15 cigarettes per day\u201d.{ref}There are dozens of articles reporting the \u201815 cigarettes a day\u2019 figure. See for example <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://www.webmd.com/balance/news/20180504/loneliness-rivals-obesity-smoking-as-health-risk\" target=\"_blank\">this in WebMD</a>, and <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://www.dailymail.co.uk/news/article-5181559/Loneliness-bad-health-smoking-15-day.html\" target=\"_blank\">this in The Daily Mail</a>. From what I gather, all the articles that cite this figure trace back to Dr. Vivek Murthy and a meta-analysis by Holt-Lunstad et al. (2015), which I discuss in more detail below in this post. When I reviewed this meta-analysis I did find the claim that loneliness is a risk factor comparable to smoking; but I could not find the calculations that lead to the 15 cigarettes benchmark. It\u2019s still unclear to me what the unit of loneliness is and what this translates into in terms of years of lives lost. However, given that 15 cigarettes per day is <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/consumption-per-smoker-per-day-bounds\" target=\"_blank\">about average consumption among smokers</a>, it\u2019s possible that the benchmark comes from comparing excess mortality of smoking for the average smoker, vs. excess mortality of loneliness for average levels of loneliness.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>It is indeed quite a shocking comparison since around <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/smoking-and-secondhand-deaths\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">7 million deaths</a> globally are attributed to smoking every year, and back-of-the-envelope calculations published in medical journals say <a href=\"https://www.bmj.com/content/bmj/320/7226/53.1.full.pdf?casa_token=Fcl9ywjt31gAAAAA:9FnQFmC50vZclWEndIVf8Vf4uwlEsKIiQeeQtFXm11_06jJwLcgZn1MS4LPyFsFVnrcCsSSHXnv5\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">one cigarette reduces your lifespan by 11 minutes</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Here we dig deeper to try to understand what the data and research tell us about the link between social relations and health. In a nutshell, my reading of the evidence is as follows:</p>\n\n\n\n<ol><li>There is a huge amount of evidence showing individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in their life.</li><li>There is credible theory and explanation of biological mechanisms, whereby isolation can set off unconscious surveillance for social threat, producing cognitive biases, reducing sleep and affecting hormones.</li><li>It’s very likely there is a causal link, but there is no credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect that loneliness has on key metrics of health, such as life expectancy.</li><li>The fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect of loneliness on health doesn’t mean we should dismiss the available evidence. But it does show that more research is needed.</li></ol>\n\n\n\n<h4>Observational studies: A first look at the data</h4>\n\n\n\n<h5>Measuring loneliness</h5>\n\n\n\n<p>Psychologists and social neuroscientists often refer to loneliness as <a href=\"https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2008-07755-000\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\"><em>painful</em> isolation</a>. The emphasis on painful is there to make a clear distinction between solitude \u2013 the state of being alone \u2013 and subjective loneliness, which is the distressing feeling that comes from unmet expectations of the types of interpersonal relationships we wish to have.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Researchers use several kinds of data to measure solitude and loneliness. The most common source of data are surveys where people are asked about different aspects of their lives, including whether they live alone, how much time they spend with other people in a given window of time (e.g. \u2018last week\u2019) or specific context (e.g. \u2018at social events, clubs or places of worship\u2019); and whether they experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. \u2018I have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with\u2019). Researchers sometimes study these survey responses separately, but often they also aggregate them in a composite index.{ref}The most popular composite index is the <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/UCLA_Loneliness_Scale\" target=\"_blank\">UCLA Loneliness Scale</a>, which was first proposed in 1978, and is based on a 20-item scale designed to measure subjective feelings of loneliness. You can read more about it <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://fetzer.org/sites/default/files/images/stories/pdf/selfmeasures/Self_Measures_for_Loneliness_and_Interpersonal_Problems_UCLA_LONELINESS.pdf\" target=\"_blank\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Surveys <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0898264312460275\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">confirm</a> that people respond differently to questions about subjective loneliness and physical social isolation, which suggests people do understand these as two distinct issues.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In the chart here I’ve put together estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness from various sources. The fact that we see such high levels of loneliness, with substantial divergence across countries, explains why this is an important and active research area. Indeed, there are literally hundreds of papers that have used survey data to explore the link between loneliness, solitude, and health. Below is an overview of what these studies find.</p>\n\n\n\n<figure><iframe style=\"width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;\" src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults\"></iframe></figure>\n\n\n\n<h5>The link between loneliness and physical health</h5>\n\n\n\n<p>Most papers studying the link between loneliness and health find that both objective solitude (e.g. living alone) and subjective loneliness (e.g. frequent self-reported feelings of loneliness) are correlated with higher morbidity (i.e. illness) and higher mortality (i.e. likelihood of death).</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The relationship between health and loneliness can of course go both ways: lonely people may see their health deteriorate with time; but it may also be the case that people who suffer from poor health end up feeling more lonely later down the line.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Because of this two-way relationship it\u2019s important to go beyond cross-sectional correlations and focus on longitudinal studies \u2013 these are studies where researchers track the same individuals over time to see if loneliness predicts illness or mortality in the future, after controlling for baseline behaviors and health status.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The evidence from longitudinal studies shows that people who experience loneliness during a period of their life tend to be more likely to have worse health later down the line. In the Netherlands, for example, <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"https://academic.oup.com/aje/article-pdf/146/6/510/455063/146-6-510.pdf\" target=\"_blank\">researchers found</a> that self-reported loneliness among adults aged 55-85 predicted mortality several months later, and this was true after controlling for age, sex, chronic diseases, alcohol use, smoking, self-assessed health condition, and functional limitations.{ref}Penninx, B. W., Van Tilburg, T., Kriegsman, D. M., Deeg, D. J., Boeke, A. J. P., & van Eijk, J. T. M. (1997). Effects of social support and personal coping resources on mortality in older age: The Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. American journal of epidemiology, 146(6), 510-519.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Most studies focus either on subjective loneliness, or on objective isolation. But some studies try to compare both. In a recent meta-analysis covering 70 longitudinal studies, the authors <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691614568352\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">write</a>:<em> \u201cWe found no differences between measures of objective and subjective social isolation. Results remain consistent across gender, length of follow-up, and world region.\u201d</em> And in the concluding section they highlight that, in their interpretation of the evidence, <em>\u201cthe risk associated with social isolation and loneliness is comparable with well-established risk factors for mortality\u201d</em>; which include smoking and obesity.{ref}Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on psychological science, 10(2), 227-237.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<h5>The link between mental health and subjective well-being</h5>\n\n\n\n<p>In another much-cited review of the evidence, Louise Hawkley and John Cacioppo, two leading experts on this topic, <a href=\"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3874845/\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">concluded</a> that <em>\u201cperhaps the most striking finding in this literature is the breadth of emotional and cognitive processes and outcomes that seem susceptible to the influence of loneliness\u201d.</em>{ref}Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of behavioral medicine, 40(2), 218-227. {/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Researchers have found that loneliness correlates with subsequent increases in symptoms related to <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/dementia-death-rates\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">dementia</a>, <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health#depression\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">depression</a>, and many other issues related to <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/mental-health\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">mental health</a>; and this holds after controlling for demographic variables, objective social isolation, stress, and baseline levels of cognitive function.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>There is also research that suggests a link between loneliness and lower <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction\" target=\"_blank\">happiness</a>, and we discuss this in more detail <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-friends\">here</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Mechanisms</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Experiments with social animals, like rats, <a href=\"https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/106/52/22393.full.pdf\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">show</a> that induced isolation can lead to a higher risk of death from cancer. Humans and rats are of course very different; but experts, such as Hawkley and Cacioppo, argue that these experiments are important because they tell us something meaningful about a shared biological mechanism. In a review of the evidence, Susan Pinker writes: \u201cIf our big brains evolved to interact, loneliness would be an early warning system\u2014a built-in alarm that sent a biological signal to members who had somehow become separated from the group\u201d.{ref}Pinker, S. (2015). The village effect: How face-to-face contact can make us healthier and happier. Vintage Canada.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Indeed, there\u2019s evidence of social regulation of gene expression in humans: <a href=\"https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/108/7/3080.full.pdf\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">studies suggest</a> perceived loneliness can switch on/off genes that regulate our immune systems, and it is this what then affects the health of humans, or other animals that evolved with similar defence mechanisms.{ref}Cole, S. W., Hawkley, L. C., Arevalo, J. M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). Transcript origin analysis identifies antigen-presenting cells as primary targets of socially regulated gene expression in leukocytes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 3080-3085.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Causality and implications</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The bulk of evidence from observational studies and biological mechanisms, described above, implies that loneliness most likely matters for our health and well being. But do we really know how much it matters relative to other important risk factors?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The key point here is that estimates are likely biased to some extent.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The findings from longitudinal studies that track individuals over time are insightful, but they cannot rule out that the relationship might be partly driven by other factors that we cannot observe. Even the studies linking loneliness and genetics can be subject to this <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"https://mru.org/dictionary-economics/omitted-variable-economics\" target=\"_blank\">omitted-variable bias</a>, because a genetic predisposition to loneliness may drive both loneliness and health outcomes.{ref}Two concrete papers that show this is a real concern are:<br>– Abdellaoui, A., Sanchez-Roige, S., Sealock, J., Treur, J. L., Dennis, J., Fontanillas, P., \u2026 & Baselmans, B. (2018). Phenome-wide investigation of health outcomes associated with genetic predisposition to loneliness. bioRxiv, 468835.<br>-Abdellaoui, A., Chen, H. Y., Willemsen, G., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Verweij, K. J., \u2026 & Cacioppo, J. T. (2019). Associations between loneliness and personality are mostly driven by a genetic association with neuroticism. Journal of personality, 87(2), 386-397.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>I could not find credible experimental evidence that would allow us to have a precise estimate of the magnitude of the causal effect.{ref}If you know of relevant studies that do provide such estimates, please let me know at [email protected].{/ref} But the fact that we struggle to pin down the magnitude of the effect doesn’t mean we should dismiss the available evidence. On the contrary \u2013 it would be great if we had evidence from randomized control trials that test positive interventions to reduce loneliness, to understand better if the \u201815 cigarettes per day\u2019 comparison from the Surgeon General of the US is roughly correct, at least for the average person.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Having a better understanding of the magnitude of the effect is important, not only because loneliness is common, but also because it\u2019s complex and unequally experienced by people around the world.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>As the chart above shows, there are large differences in self-reported loneliness across countries. We should understand how important these differences are for the distribution of health and well-being.</p>\n\n\n\n<h3>Are we happier when we spend more time with others?</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>In 1938, a group of Harvard researchers decided to start a research program to track the lives of a group of young men, in what eventually became one of the longest and most famous longitudinal studies of its kind. The idea was to track the development of a group of teenage boys through periodic interviews and medical checkups, with the aim of understanding how their health and well-being evolved as they grew up.{ref}For historical context: The Harvard Graduate School of Education \u2013 the first to admit women \u2013 opened admissions to women in 1920; and the Harvard Medical School accepted its first female enrollees in 1945, even though the first woman applied in 1847. You can read more about the hard-earned gains for women at Harvard here: <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/\" target=\"_blank\">https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2012/04/hard-earned-gains-for-women-at-harvard/</a>{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Today, more than 80 years later, it is one of the longest running research programmes in social science. It is called the <a href=\"https://www.adultdevelopmentstudy.org/\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">Harvard Study of Adult Development</a> and it is still running. The program started with 724 boys, and researchers continue to monitor today the health and well-being of those initial participants who are still alive, most in their late 90s.{ref}The original participants for this study came from two groups: a cohort of 268 men, 19 years of age, from the Harvard classes of 1939-1944; and a group of 456 caucasian men, ages 11-16, from underprivileged neighborhoods of Boston. Over the years the project evolved, enrolling more participants, and many of the children of the original participants are included in the study today.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This is a unique scientific exercise \u2013 most longitudinal studies do not last this long because too many participants drop out, researchers move on to other projects (or even die), or the funding dries up.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>So what have we learned from this unique study?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Robert Waldinger, the current director of the study, summarized \u2013 in what is now one of the <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://www.ted.com/talks/robert_waldinger_what_makes_a_good_life_lessons_from_the_longest_study_on_happiness?language=en\" target=\"_blank\">most viewed TED Talks to date</a> \u2013 the findings from decades of research. The main result, he concluded, is that social connections are one of the most important factors for people\u2019s <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction\" target=\"_blank\">happiness</a> and <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/health-meta\" target=\"_blank\">health</a>. He said: \u201cThose who kept warm relationships got to live longer and happier, and the loners often died earlier.\u201d</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Here I take a closer look at the evidence and show more research that finds a consistent link between social connections and happiness. But before we get to the details, let me explain why I think this link is important.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>As most people can attest from personal experience, striving for happiness is not easy. In fact, the search for happiness can become a source of <em>unhappiness</em> \u2013 there are studies that show actively pursuing happiness <a href=\"https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">can end up decreasing it</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The data shows that <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#the-link-between-happiness-and-income\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noreferrer noopener\">income and happiness are clearly related</a>; but we also know from surveys that people often <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/spc3.12386\" target=\"_blank\">overestimate the impact of income on happiness</a>. Social relations might be the missing link: In rich countries, where minimum material living conditions are <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/extreme-poverty#historical-poverty-in-today-s-rich-countries\" target=\"_blank\">often satisfied</a>, people may struggle to become happier because they are targeting material rather than social goals.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>The cross-sectional correlation between happiness and friends</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/wvs.jsp\" target=\"_blank\">World Values Survey (WVS)</a> is a large cross-country research project that collects data from a series of representative national surveys. In the fourth wave (1999-2004) the WVS asked respondents hundreds of questions about their lives, including whether they were part of social or religious groups, how often they spent time with friends, and whether they felt happy with their life.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>By comparing self-reported happiness among those with and without frequent social interactions, we can get an idea of whether there is indeed a raw correlation between happiness and social relations across different societies.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In the chart I show this comparison: The green points correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends at least once per month; while the blue dots correspond to happiness among those who interact with friends less often.{ref}The question on frequency of interaction with friends was asked in the context of time spent at work, clubs or places of worship. The options included \u2018weekly\u2019, \u2018once or twice a month\u2019, \u2018only a few times a month\u2019, and \u2018not at all\u2019. The options for happiness, on the other hand, included \u2018very happy\u2019, \u2018quite happy\u2019, \u2018not very happy\u2019 and \u2018not at all happy\u2019. In the chart I took this data, and I split the sample in two: I classified respondents as \u2018often meeting with friends\u2019 if they said they met with friends once a month or more frequently; and I classified them as \u2018rarely meeting with friends\u2019 if they said they met with friends \u2018once a year\u2019 or \u2018not at all\u2019. Then, for each of these two groups, I plotted the share who reported being \u2018very happy\u2019 or ‘quite happy\u2019.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This chart shows that in almost all countries, people who often spend time with their friends report to be happier than those who spend less time with friends.{ref}The estimates have of course a margin of error that varies with the survey size. The difference between the green and blue dots is statistically significant for about half of the countries (i.e. for about half of the countries in this chart, the 95% confidence intervals around the blue and green dots would not overlap). {/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<div class=\"wp-block-image\"><figure class=\"aligncenter\"><a href=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/07/happiness-and-friends-v3.png\"><img loading=\"lazy\" width=\"800\" height=\"1132\" src=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/07/happiness-and-friends-v3-e1564090552891.png\" alt=\"\" class=\"wp-image-23997\"/></a></figure></div>\n\n\n\n<h4>The link between social relations and happiness over time</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart above gives a cross-sectional perspective \u2013 it\u2019s just a snapshot that compares different people at a given point in time. What happens if we look at changes in social relations and happiness over time?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>There is a large academic literature in medicine and psychology that shows individuals who report feelings of loneliness are more likely to have health problems later in life (you can read more about this in my <a rel=\"noopener noreferrer\" href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being\" target=\"_blank\">post on social relations and health</a>); and similarly, there are also many studies that show that changes in social relations predict changes in happiness and life satisfaction.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>One of the research papers that draws on data from the famous Harvard Study of Adult Development, for example, looked at the experiences of 82 married participants and their spouses, and found that greater self-reported couple attachment predicted lower levels of depression and greater life satisfaction 2.5 years later.{ref}Waldinger, R. J., Cohen, S., Schulz, M. S., & Crowell, J. A. (2015). Security of attachment to spouses in late life: Concurrent and prospective links with cognitive and emotional well-being. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(4), 516-529. Available online <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4579537/\" target=\"_blank\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p> Other studies with larger population samples have also found a similar cross-temporal link: perceived social isolation predicts subsequent changes in depressive symptoms but not vice versa, and this holds after controlling for demographic variables and stress.{ref}See for example:<br>– Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2010). Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and aging, 25(2), 453.<br>– Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., & Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychology and aging, 21(1), 140.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Searching for happiness is typically an intentional and active pursuit. Is it the case that people tend to become happier when they purposely decide to improve their social relations?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This is a tough empirical question to test; but <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0956797618761660\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">a recent study</a> found evidence pointing in this direction.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Using a large representative survey in Germany, researchers asked participants to report, in text format, ideas for how they could improve their life satisfaction. Based on these answers, the researchers then investigated which types of ideas predicted changes in life satisfaction one year later.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The researchers found that those who reported socially-engaged strategies (e.g. \u201cI plan to spend more time with friends and family\u201d) often reported improvements in life satisfaction one year later; while those who described other non-social active pursuits (e.g. \u201cI plan to find a better job\u201d) did not report increased life satisfaction.{ref}The researchers used an open question: \u201cWhat could you do to ensure that you will be more satisfied in the future?\u201d. There are of course many types of answers that people can provide to such an open-ended question, so the authors decided to classify each individual answer into one of two categories, depending on whether or not the respondents described a \u2018socially engaged strategy\u2019. This process of classifying answers could naturally raise questions: the authors could manipulate the subjective classification of answers to fit their favourite hypothesis. In order to insure themselves against this criticism, the researchers relied on three independent coders, and they stipulated the classification method and the expected results in a <a rel=\"noreferrer noopener\" href=\"https://blogs.worldbank.org/impactevaluations/a-pre-analysis-plan-checklist\" target=\"_blank\">pre-analysis plan</a>. You find more details in the paper: Rohrer, J. M., Richter, D., Br\u00fcmmer, M., Wagner, G. G., & Schmukle, S. C. (2018). Successfully striving for happiness: Socially engaged pursuits predict increases in life satisfaction. Psychological science, 29(8), 1291-1298.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Are we happier when we spend more time with others?</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>From decades of research we know that social relations predict mental well-being over time; and from a recent study we also know that people who actively decide to improve their social relations often report becoming happier. So yes, people are happier when they spend more time with friends.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Does this mean that if we have an exogenous shock to our social relations this will have a permanent negative effect on our happiness?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>We can\u2019t really answer this with the available evidence. More research is needed to really understand the causal mechanisms that drive the link between happiness and social relations.{ref}The key point to keep in mind is that we are not talking about experimental evidence. People who frequently interact with friends, or people who say they want to engage more in \u2018social strategies to improve their lives\u2019 are likely different; and although the researchers do try to control for this, it is very hard to measure all the relevant characteristics that might help drive life satisfaction. Similarly, asking people about strategies to improve their lives is difficult, and although the qualitative strategy from the paper above is interesting, it\u2019s still hard to draw accurate comparisons: it\u2019s possible that \u2018non-social\u2019 strategies, for example, are more difficult or stressful to pursue; so part of the lower apparent impact that these strategies have on happiness might come from the fact that some people are trying but failing to find a better job.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Despite this, however, I think that we should take the available observational evidence seriously. In a way, the causal impact from a random shock is less interesting than the evidence from active strategies that people might take to improve their happiness. People who get divorced, for example, often report a short-term drop in life satisfaction; but over time they tend to recover and eventually end up being more satisfied with their life than shortly before they divorced (you can read more about this in our <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/happiness-and-life-satisfaction#life-satisfaction-through-life-events\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noreferrer noopener\">entry on happiness and life satisfaction</a>).</p>\n\n\n\n<p>It makes sense to consider the possibility that healthy social relationships are a key missing piece for human wellbeing. Among other things, this would help explain the paradoxical result from studies where actively pursuing happiness <a href=\"https://www.ocf.berkeley.edu/~eerlab/pdf/papers/Ford_Mauss_Pursuit_of_Positive_Emotion.pdf\" target=\"_blank\" rel=\"noopener noreferrer\">apparently ends up decreasing it</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<h3>The importance of social networks for innovation and productivity</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>Economies today are increasingly built on ideas. Consider, for example, commercial aviation. After a flight, <em>Southwest Airlines </em>can unload and reload passengers to get the same airliner back in the air in about 10 minutes \u2013 this is half the time the same process took in the 1970s. As the economist John Cochrane <a href=\"https://review.chicagobooth.edu/economics/2019/article/what-success-rock-climbing-tells-us-about-economic-growth\">explains</a>, this achievement is as much about ideas and know-how as it is about technology. <em>Southwest Airlines</em>, and many other carriers after them, have been able to expand their output because they now have <a href=\"https://www.npr.org/2015/06/28/418147961/the-man-who-saved-southwest-airlines-with-a-10-minute-idea\">more efficient protocols</a> for \u2018turning planes\u2019 between flights.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In 2018 Paul Romer won the Nobel Prize in Economics \u201cfor integrating technological innovations into long-run macroeconomic analysis\u201d. Romer first fleshed out this observation in his <a href=\"http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf\">seminal 1990 paper</a>, titled \u201cEndogenous Technological Change\u201d. At the heart of his contributions was precisely this observation: the diffusion of ideas fosters economic and productivity growth.{ref}Romer\u2019s seminal paper, with nearly 30,000 citations in Google Scholar is Romer, P. M. (1990). Endogenous technological change. Journal of Political Economy, 98 (5, Part 2), S71-S102. Available online <a href=\"http://web.stanford.edu/~klenow/Romer_1990.pdf\">here</a>. After this paper he wrote many other related articles elaborating on the same ideas. See, for example, Romer, P. M. (1992). Two strategies for economic development: using ideas and producing ideas. The World Bank Economic Review, 6(suppl_1), 63-91. Available online <a href=\"https://academic.oup.com/wber/article-abstract/6/suppl_1/63/1656015?redirectedFrom=fulltext\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Romer\u2019s theory of \u201cendogenous growth\u201d \u2013 placing ideas at the heart of economic technologies \u2013 made economists ask entirely new questions about the drivers of growth. Where do ideas come from? How do ideas spread? </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Economists have asked these questions of the data, and their findings suggest social networks play a key role. Friendship and professional ties matter for economic growth because people tend to develop new ideas by interacting and learning from others who are close to them.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In a nutshell, social networks facilitate the diffusion of ideas across individuals and firms, and because of this, they play an important role in productivity growth. Here\u2019s how this works.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Knowledge spillovers, social networks, and patent citations</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Economic theory suggests we should expect social connections to have an effect on productivity via \u201cknowledge spillovers”. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>The basic idea is that social connections make it easier for knowledge to travel farther and faster. Knowledge is different from many physical assets, in that it can be used by many people at the same time; so the diffusion of knowledge creates productivity gains that spillover through society. As Thomas Jefferson once said: <em>\u201cHe who receives an idea from me, receives instruction himself without lessening mine; as he who lights his taper at mine, receives light without darkening me\u201d.</em>{ref}Letter to Isaac McPherson. August 13, 1813. Online <a href=\"https://en.wikisource.org/wiki/Letter_to_Isaac_McPherson_-_August_13,_1813\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>What is the empirical value of this theory? What do \u2018knowledge spillovers\u2019 actually look like in the real world? Do scientists and engineers really rely on social connections to disseminate their new ideas and inventions?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>To answer these questions economists have looked into historical data from <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patent-applications-per-million\">patents</a>. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Patents are government licences used to register the intellectual property of new inventions, ranging <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/patents-usa-by-category\">from machines to medical drugs</a>. When a patent application is filed, inventors are required to include a list of citations, in which they describe how their technological development builds upon the results of earlier patents. These historical records of patent citations give us an overview of how knowledge flows \u2013 patent citations are the \u2018paper trail\u2019 left by ideas as they travel, evolve, and make new ideas possible.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>One of the first analyses of patent citations in the context of knowledge spillovers was done by Adam Jaffe, Manuel Trajtenberg and Rebecca Henderson, in <a href=\"https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813\">a much-cited paper</a> published in the Quarterly Journal of Economics in 1993.{ref}Jaffe, A. B., Trajtenberg, M., & Henderson, R. (1993). Geographic localization of knowledge spillovers as evidenced by patent citations. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 108(3), 577-598. Available online <a href=\"https://academic.oup.com/qje/article/108/3/577/1881813\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Jaffe and coauthors studied, in particular, the geographic distribution of citation patterns in the US. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>If social connections did not matter for new ideas and growth, then we should expect that new inventions are just as likely to cite patents from far away as they are to cite the work of their neighbours. In contrast, Jaffe and coauthors found that new patents were disproportionately more likely to cite previous <em>local</em> patents. In other words, they found that patent citations were strongly geographically concentrated. According to their estimates, citations were three to four times as likely to come from the same state as the originating patent \u2013 this is consistent with the idea that social connections and proximity do indeed matter for innovation.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>After this initial study, several other papers have used patent citations to explore the determinants of knowledge spillovers, with a broader perspective on \u2018proximity\u2019. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Ajay Agrawal, Devesh Kapur and John McHale used the ethnicity of researchers as a proxy for social connectedness, and <a href=\"http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.515.3876&rep=rep1&type=pdf\">found</a> that co-location and co-ethnicity both increase the probability of knowledge flow between inventors. Co-location increases the probability of a knowledge flow by 24% (assuming non-co-ethnic inventors) and co-ethnicity increases the probability by 14% (assuming non-co-located inventors). Again, these large concentration patterns seem consistent with the idea that proximity and social networks matter for innovation.{ref} Agrawal, A., Kapur, D., & McHale, J. (2008). How do spatial and social proximity influence knowledge flows? Evidence from patent data. Journal of Urban Economics, 64(2), 258-269.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Disentangling geographic proximity and social interactions</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The correlation between proximity and patent citations partly captures the effect that local factors may have on innovation (e.g. geography, regulation or infrastructure around industry hubs where similar workers and firms concentrate). However, the evidence shows that social connections matter for the transmission of knowledge even after we control for these other factors.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In <a href=\"https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259\">a paper</a> published in 2018 in the <em>Journal of Economic Perspectives</em>, Michael Bailey, Rachel Cao, Theresa Kuchler, Johannes Stroebel, and Arlene Wong combined patent records with Facebook data from the US, and found that social connectedness between US counties (measured by the frequency of Facebook ties between counties) predicts the probability of cross-county patent citations. This is true, even after accounting for the patents\u2019 technology class and geographic distance between counties.{ref} The full reference of the paper is: Bailey, M., Cao, R., Kuchler, T., Stroebel, J., & Wong, A. (2018). Social connectedness: measurement, determinants, and effects. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 32(3), 259-80. Available online <a href=\"https://pubs.aeaweb.org/doi/pdf/10.1257/jep.32.3.259\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This means that the probability that an inventor builds on the work of another inventor in a different part of the country can be partly explained by how well-connected their social backgrounds are, even after controlling for geographic distance and other demographic factors.{ref}To be more precise, Bailey and co-authors find that the effect of doubling social connectedness on the probability of citation remains significant and large, at 4.9 percent, even after controlling for geographic distance. When they add controls for cross-country differences on socioeconomic indicators (income, education, ethnicity, etc.) they actually find that the estimated relationship between social connectedness and patent citation increases somewhat as a result of these further controls. This finding suggests that the relationship between geographic distance and the probability of patent citation, viewed in isolation, may be partially capturing effects of information flows associated with social connectedness.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The implications of this result are of great consequence in the age of the internet. The development of new communication-enabling technologies, such as <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media\">the rise of social media</a>, have opened new possibilities for creating and maintaining social connections, and this is likely to have had a positive effect on the transmission of knowledge.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Social connections and (material) well-being</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Globally, the total economic output per person today is <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&tab=chart&year=1950&time=1870..latest&country=OWID_WRL\">4.4-times larger</a> than in 1950 (even after accounting for inflation). This was achieved over a period during which people tended towards working <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/annual-working-hours-per-worker?tab=chart&stackMode=absolute&time=1950..2017&country=BRA~COL~FRA~DEU~NLD~GBR~USA&region=World\" data-type=\"URL\" data-id=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/annual-working-hours-per-worker?tab=chart&stackMode=absolute&time=1950..2017&country=BRA~COL~FRA~DEU~NLD~GBR~USA&region=World\">fewer hours</a>. We now produce more whilst working less.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>As our interactive chart here shows, this historical achievement is underpinned by productivity growth: in many countries, workers produce much higher output per hour today than in 1950.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This is partly because of new machines and technologies, but also because we have new shared knowledge about how to work more efficiently.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The research on social connections and innovation suggests that one important way to improve material living standards is to invest in digital communication technologies. This lowers the costs of creating and maintaining personal and professional ties, which facilitates the diffusion of ideas and knowledge, creating positive productivity spillovers.</p>\n\n\n\n<figure><iframe src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/labor-productivity-per-hour-PennWorldTable?time=1950..2017&country=BRA+DEU+IND+JPN+KOR+GBR+USA\"></iframe></figure>\n\n\n\t<block type=\"additional-information\" default-open=\"false\">\n\t\t<content>\n\n<h3>Technical Note:</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>Social networks are diverse and complex, so talking about \u201csocial connectedness\u201d is actually a simplification that deserves some discussion. Researchers studying social networks have shown that the flow of information and knowledge through people is not only a function of \u201cconnectedness\u201d as measured by the number of links people have, but also a function of the specific structures of the networks (e.g. the extent to which social ties are tightly clustered). </p>\n\n\n\n<p>For the diffusion of information, the concept of \u2018homophily\u2019, for example, is key. <br>Homophily is the tendency of agents to associate with other agents who have similar characteristics. This manifests itself in many ways \u2013 for example, in the fact that friendship ties are typically strongly related to ethnicity. If one is interested in how information spreads through social learning, patterns such as homophily or social segregation are important to understand. For example, in the labor market, homophily isolates workers of one ethnicity from workers of another ethnicity, which then limits the extent to which individuals in one group hear about openings and opportunities known to the other group.<br>Social networks research in this area shows that, in general, as homophily increases, the propensity for a diffusion to gain hold within a particular group rises, sometimes at the expense of the speed and extent of diffusion throughout the entire population. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Similarly, diversity may play a role. Observational studies <a href=\"https://science.sciencemag.org/content/328/5981/1029\">show</a> that the diversity of individuals\u2019 relationships is strongly correlated with the economic development of communities.<br>You can read more about this in Jackson, M. O., Rogers, B. W., & Zenou, Y. (2017). The economic consequences of social-network structure. Journal of Economic Literature, 55(1), 49-95. Available online <a href=\"https://www.econstor.eu/bitstream/10419/175053/1/Wp1116.pdf\">here</a>.</p>\n\n</content>\n\t</block>\n\n\n<h3>The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, West German families that had relatives in East Germany saw an increase in their income, and this growth in incomes was <a href=\"https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf\">significantly higher</a> than that of comparable West German families without such family ties to the East. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Why did this happen? Are personal and family ties really so important to our economic outcomes that they result in systematic differences in income growth?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Researchers have studied this question and have found empirical support for the idea that social connections do, indeed, have large effects on economic outcomes. </p>\n\n\n\n<h4>A natural experiment: Friends and incomes after the fall of the Berlin Wall</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Measuring the impact of personal relations on economic outcomes is difficult. This is because these two aspects of our life are often closely intertwined \u2013 social ties facilitate economic interactions, but at the same time people with whom we interact for economic reasons end up becoming acquaintances or even friends. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>To overcome this difficulty, the economists Konrad Burchardi and Tarek Hassan studied a historical event that suddenly opened up new economic interactions, where previously only non-economic social connections were possible: The fall of the Berlin Wall.{ref}Konrad B. Burchardi and Tarek A. Hassan. “The economic impact of social ties: Evidence from German reunification.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 128.3 (2013): 1219-1271. Available online <a href=\"https://www.nber.org/papers/w17186.pdf\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>For decades private economic exchange between East and West Germany was virtually impossible, but at the same time people on both sides maintained social ties for non-economic reasons. After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989, trade and collaboration between the two Germanies suddenly became feasible. Burchardi and Hassan studied the impact that this sudden change had on household incomes.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>After the fall of the Berlin Wall in 1989 there was <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/maddison-data-gdp-per-capita-in-2011us?tab=chart&time=1500..2018&country=~DEU\">growth in incomes</a> across Germany as a whole; but the interesting finding from Burchardi and Hassan is that income growth for households in West Germany who had ties to at least one relative in the East was much higher \u2013 six percentage points higher \u2013 than that of comparable households without such ties.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Burchardi and Hassan argue that West German households that had ties to East Germany had a comparative advantage in seizing the new economic opportunities in the East. Having personal relationships with East Germans gave them access to valuable economic information \u2013 information regarding local demand conditions, and about the quality of East German assets that were offered to investors.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>These gains from social connections actually added up at the regional level. West German regions that had a higher concentration of households with social ties to the East, enjoyed substantially higher growth in incomes in the early 1990s. A one standard deviation rise in the share of households with social ties to East Germany in 1989 was associated with a 4.6 percentage point rise in income per capita over six years.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Personal relations and job search: Evidence from Europe</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The unique situation of the German reunification shows that social ties that are initially unrelated to economic interests can become important for economic success.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The key mechanism is that personal connections allow us to seize new economic opportunities. This may sound abstract, but in fact most of us have experienced this in our own lives. Perhaps the best example is job searching: personal contacts and connections are one of the most common ways for people to find a job.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In the book <em>The Human Network</em>, the economist Matthew Jackson draws on decades of research to help explain how social connections affect power, beliefs and behaviors. With regard to job networks and social capital, he writes: \u201c<em>If you have ever struggled to find a job, you are not alone. Without well-connected friends or family it is hard to make it in any industry […]. Finding a job without having some personal connection to someone already employed at a firm is more the exception than the rule.\u201d </em>{ref}Pages 239-241 in Jackson, M. O. (2019). <em>The Human Network: How Your Social Position Determines Your Power, Beliefs, and Behaviors</em>. Pantheon.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Our interactive chart here shows evidence for Jackson\u2019s claim using data from European countries. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart shows the distribution of job search methods by country. Data for Spain is shown by default, but you can explore data for different countries using the option \u2018Change country\u2019.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In Spain, 84% of the unemployed rely on contacts \u2013 friends, relatives, trade unions \u2013 to find a job. It is the most common job-search method. Direct applications and ads rank below, on places two and three.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>If you plot the same data for other countries you\u2019ll see that personal contacts are a very important job-search method everywhere in Europe, but there is some variation across countries. At the lowest end of the spectrum, in Belgium, about 38% of people rely on contacts to find a job; at the other end, in Greece and Latvia, 90% of job seekers rely on this method.</p>\n\n\n\n<p><em>(NB. </em><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-friends-relatives-trade-unions-in-job-search?tab=map\"><em>This interactive map</em></a><em> shows the same data, but focusing specifically on the proportion of job seekers relying on contacts for job search across Europe.)</em></p>\n\n\n\n<figure><iframe src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/proportion-of-unemployed-using-specific-search-methods\"></iframe></figure>\n\n\n\n<h4>Friends and relatives remain a popular method for finding a job, even in the age of digital job search platforms </h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart showing job search methods across Europe relies on data from the European Labor Force Survey, 2006-2008. We have not found more recent data that allows cross-country comparisons, but surveys from individual high-income countries show that this remains relevant today, even after the expansion of online job search engines, and <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/rise-of-social-media\">the rise of social media platforms</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In the US, data from the Pew Research Center shows that the internet is the most important resource for job search, but 66% of survey respondents still say they relied on connections with close friends or family in their most recent search for a job. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>These two methods are of course not mutually exclusive, and the boundaries between \u2018internet\u2019 and \u2018personal contact\u2019 are becoming less clear as job search platforms become more integrated with social media.</p>\n\n\n\n<figure><iframe src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/job-seeker-job-search-pew\"></iframe></figure>\n\n\n\n<h4>Personal relations matter for trade</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Personal relations can also provide information and social collateral that can be valuable for economic interactions across national borders.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The political scientist Anna Lee Saxenian <a href=\"https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf\">analyzed</a> the biographies of Chinese and Indian engineers who migrated to California in the 1970s. She found that as these skilled immigrants excelled in their personal careers, they started leveraging their social ties to relatives and friends in their home regions. This allowed them to outsource important operations for a large fraction of Silicon Valley firms.{ref}Saxenian, A. (2000). Silicon Valley\u2019s new immigrant entrepreneurs. UC San DiegoWorking Papers. The Center for Comparative Immigration Studies. Available online <a href=\"https://escholarship.org/content/qt88x6505q/qt88x6505q.pdf\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Saxenian argues that by connecting Silicon Valley firms to low-cost, high-quality labor in India and China, they became instrumental for the success of Silicon Valley and crucial in the emergence of their home regions as major hubs of the global IT services industry.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Personal relations matter for <em>collective</em> economic outcomes</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>In some situations personal connections may facilitate economic transactions for one person (or group of people) at the expense of another one. This is what many people have in mind when they think about job referrals. If there is only one job vacancy, giving the job to a friend means not giving the job to someone else. If social connections become a vehicle for favouritism, this can lead to inefficiencies.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In many contexts, however, the opposite is true. Social connections often have a net-positive effect on aggregate economic outcomes because much of the impact of social connections on economic transactions is not zero-sum \u2013 for example, when social connections reduce information frictions and allow people to learn from each other. As we discuss in a <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/social-networks-innovation-and-productivity\">companion post</a>, there is empirical evidence showing these knowledge spillovers are substantial.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Social networks can affect economic outcomes via agglomeration effects</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The concentration of firms and workers in \u2018industry hubs\u2019 is partly explained by the fact that social connections create economic externalities: Agglomeration leads to denser social connections, and this has positive spillover effects on productivity.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Again the fall of the Berlin Wall offers clear evidence of this.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>When Berlin was split, economic activity in West Berlin shifted away from the historic business district, known as \u2018Mitte\u2019. As a consequence property prices there fell relative to the new economic areas that emerged further away from the Wall. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>When the Wall came down the distribution of economic activity within the city changed quickly once again: The old business district began to re-emerge and economic activity shifted back to the historic center. This is the conclusion of a careful econometric study, published by Gabriel Ahlfeldt, Stephen Redding, Daniel Sturm and Nikolaus Wolf in the journal <em>Econometrica</em>.{ref} Ahlfeldt, G. M., Redding, S. J., Sturm, D. M., & Wolf, N. (2015). The economics of density: Evidence from the Berlin Wall. Econometrica, 83(6), 2127-2189. Online <a href=\"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.3982/ECTA10876\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The three maps show the evolution of land prices in Berlin. These maps, from Ahlfeldt and co-authors, illustrate graphically the impact that the Wall had on the geography of economic activity. The vertical scale measures relative land prices; this means higher elevation, highlighted with darker shades, denotes higher prices.{ref} The authors use the same vertical scale for each figure, and land prices are normalized to have a mean of 1 in each year, so the levels of the land price surfaces in each map are comparable.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The map on the left shows that in 1936 there are two areas with very high land values in West Berlin and both are close to the center (they are both part of a concentric ring around the pre-war central business district, located in the middle).</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In 1986, we see smaller peak values; in fact, we see that the pre-war peak closest to the Wall is entirely eliminated following division, as this area ceased to be an important center of commercial and retail activity. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Finally, in 2006, we see again higher peaks; and we see that the area closest to the Wall is reemerging as a center of high land values. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>This evolution is a great example of the economics of density: The areas in West Berlin close to the old central business district became less productive during the division but regained their productivity and attractiveness again soon after reunification. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Knowledge, public resources, and opportunities for trade and collaboration that had been inaccessible for decades, suddenly became available after the fall of the Wall.{ref} Ahlfeldt and coauthors disentangle agglomeration effects from other forces that took place at the same time. In particular, they account for the fact that the Berlin Wall had an impact both on agglomeration forces (i.e. \u201cproduction externalities\u201d determined by the surrounding density of workers); as well as on “production fundamentals” (i.e. the physical characteristics of the different neighborhoods where workers and firms concentrate).{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<figure class=\"wp-block-image\"><img loading=\"lazy\" width=\"800\" height=\"389\" src=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/11/berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015-800x389.png\" alt=\"\" class=\"wp-image-27847\" srcset=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/11/berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015-800x389.png 800w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/11/berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015-150x73.png 150w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/11/berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015-400x194.png 400w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/11/berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015-768x373.png 768w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/11/berlin-land-prices-Ahlfeldt-etal-2015.png 1459w\" sizes=\"(max-width: 800px) 100vw, 800px\" /></figure>\n\n\n\n<h4>The importance of personal relations for economic outcomes</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Social connections matter hugely for economic outcomes. Personal relations, even those that we maintain for non-economic reasons, often give us access to information and provide us with social collateral for economic transactions \u2013 from buying a house to getting a job. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>This can have positive consequences that extend beyond the individuals directly involved in the transactions. When personal and professional ties facilitate the exchange of information and knowledge, this can bring benefits to society as a whole. The implication from this is that social connections are not only important because they <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being\">affect our emotional well-being</a>, but also because, as we show here, they affect our <em>material</em> well-being too.</p>\n\n\n\n<h2>Loneliness, solitude and social isolation</h2>\n\n\n\n<h3>Living alone is becoming increasingly common around the world</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>In the US, the share of adults who live alone nearly doubled over the last 50 years. This is not only happening in the US: single-person households have become increasingly common in many countries across the world, from Angola to Japan.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Historical records show that this \u2018rise of living alone\u2019 started in early-industrialized countries over a century ago, accelerating around 1950. In countries such as Norway and Sweden, single-person households were rare a century ago, but today they account for nearly half of all households. In some cities they are already the majority.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Surveys and census data from recent decades show that people are more likely to live alone in rich countries, and the prevalence of single-person households is unprecedented historically. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Social connections \u2013 including contact with friends and family \u2013 are <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being\">important</a> for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as single-person households become more common, there will be new challenges to connect and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where welfare states are weaker.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>But it\u2019s important to keep things in perspective. It\u2019s unhelpful to compare the rise of living alone with a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019, which is what newspaper articles often write in <a href=\"https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA\">alarming headlines</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Loneliness and solitude are <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone\">not the same</a>, and the evidence suggests that self-reported loneliness has <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic\"><em>not</em> been growing</a> in recent decades.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Historical perspective on people living alone: Evidence from rich countries </h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Historical records of inhabitants across villages and cities in today\u2019s rich countries give us insights into how uncommon it was for people to live alone in the past.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart here, adapted from <a href=\"https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true\">a paper</a> by the historian Keith Snell, shows estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data. Each dot corresponds to an estimate for one settlement in Europe, North America, Japan or Britain.{ref}Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28. Online <a href=\"https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093?needAccess=true\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The share of one-person households remained fairly steady between the early modern period and through the 19th century \u2013 typically below 10%. Then growth started in the twentieth century, accelerating in the 1960s.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The current prevalence of one-person households is unprecedented historically. The highest point recorded in this chart corresponds to Stockholm, in 2012, where 60% of households consist of one person.</p>\n\n\n\n<figure class=\"wp-block-image size-large\"><img loading=\"lazy\" width=\"652\" height=\"550\" src=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/historical-one-person-households-652x550.png\" alt=\"\" class=\"wp-image-28961\" srcset=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/historical-one-person-households-652x550.png 652w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/historical-one-person-households-400x338.png 400w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/historical-one-person-households-150x127.png 150w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/historical-one-person-households-768x648.png 768w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/historical-one-person-households.png 1358w\" sizes=\"(max-width: 652px) 100vw, 652px\" /></figure>\n\n\n\n<h4>The rise of one-person households across the world</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>For recent decades, census data can be combined with data from large cross-country surveys, to provide a global perspective on the proportion of households with only one member (i.e. the proportion of single-person households). This gives us a proxy for the prevalence of solitary living arrangements.{ref}The percentage of single-person households is a proxy for \u2018solitary living\u2019. However, it\u2019s important to emphasise that these two things are not really the same. Single-person households include those where a person lives alone in an individual housing unit, but they also include people who live independently as lodgers in a separate room within a larger housing unit with other occupant. So technically, there are some people who live in a \u2018single-person household\u2019, but they don\u2019t really live alone.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>We produced this chart combining individual reports from statistical country offices, cross-country surveys such as the <a href=\"https://dhsprogram.com/\"><em>Demographic and Health Surveys</em></a>, and estimates published in the EU\u2019s <em><a href=\"https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat\">Eurostat</a></em>, the UN\u2019s <a href=\"https://unstats.un.org/unsd/demographic-social/products/dyb/\">Demographic Year Books</a>, and the <a href=\"https://figshare.com/articles/German_Time_Series_Dataset_1834_2012/1450809/1\">Deutschland in Daten</a> dataset. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart shows that the trend of rising single-person households extends across all world regions. There are large differences between countries \u2013 from more than 40% in northern European countries to 1% in low-income Asian countries.</p>\n\n\n\n<p><em>(NB. For the US and Canada there are long-run time series from census data that let us directly track the share of people who live alone. This is shown in </em><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/adults-living-alone\"><em>this other chart</em></a><em>, where you can see the same trend.) </em><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households\"></a></p>\n\n\n\n<iframe src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households\" style=\"width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;\"></iframe>\n\n\n\n<h4>Living arrangements and prosperity</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>National income per capita and the share of one-person households are strongly correlated: As the chart here shows, people are more likely to live alone in rich countries.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In this interactive chart you can move the slider to see changes over time. This reveals that the rise of single-person households tends to be larger in countries where GDP per capita has grown more. <em>(NB. You can also see the correlation over time in </em><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?stackMode=relative&time=1990..2016\"><em>this other</em></a><em> scatter plot comparing average growth in GDP vs average growth in one-person households).</em></p>\n\n\n\n<p>These correlations are partly due to the fact that people who can afford to, often choose to live alone. Indeed, rising incomes in many countries are likely part of the reason why people are more likely to live alone today than in the past. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>But there must be more to it since even at the same level of incomes there are clear differences between regions. In particular, Asian countries have systematically fewer one-person households than African countries with comparable GDP levels. Ghana and Pakistan, for example, have similar GDP per capita, but in Pakistan one-person households are extremely rare, while in Ghana they are common (about 1 in 4). This suggests culture and country-specific factors also play an important role.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Additionally, there are other non-cultural country-specific factors that are likely to play a role. In particular, rich countries often have more extensive social support networks, so people in these countries find it easier to take risks. Living alone is more risky in poorer countries, because there\u2019s often less supply of services and infrastructure to support more solitary living arrangements. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>And finally, it\u2019s also likely that some of the causality runs in the opposite direction. It\u2019s not only that incomes, culture or welfare states enable people to live alone, but also that for many workers attaining higher incomes in today\u2019s economy often demands changes in living arrangements. Migration<a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/urbanization\"> from rural to urban areas</a> is the prime example.</p>\n\n\n\n<iframe src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita\" style=\"width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;\"></iframe>\n\n\n\n<h4>Is the rise of one-person households a problem? </h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Social connections \u2013 including contact with friends and family \u2013 are <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being\">important</a> for our health and emotional well-being. Hence, as the \u2018rise of living alone\u2019 continues, there will be new challenges to connect people and provide support to those living alone, particularly in poorer countries where communication technologies are <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/internet\">less developed</a> and welfare states are <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/government-spending#high-income-countries-spend-more-on-social-protection-than-low-income-countries\">weaker</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>But, it\u2019s also important to keep in mind that living alone is <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone\">not the same</a> as feeling lonely. There\u2019s evidence that living alone is, by itself, a poor predictor of loneliness. Self-reported loneliness has <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/loneliness-epidemic\">not been growing </a>in recent decades, and in fact, the countries where people are most likely to say they have support from family and friends, are the same countries \u2013 in Scandinavia \u2013 where a large fraction of the population lives alone.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Incomes and freedom of choice are not the only drivers of the \u2018rise of living alone\u2019; but it would be remiss to ignore they do contribute to this trend.</p>\n\n\n\n<p><a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/economic-growth\">Higher incomes</a>, <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/structural-transformation-and-deindustrialization-evidence-from-todays-rich-countries\">economic transitions</a> that enable migration from agriculture in rural areas into manufacturing and services in cities, and <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/female-labor-supply\">rising female participation in labor markets</a> all play a role. People are more likely to live alone today than in the past partly because they are increasingly able to do so.<br></p>\n\n\n\n<h3>Loneliness and aloneness are not the same</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>There is a popular <a href=\"https://www.vice.com/sv/article/mvn7eb/the-swedish-theory-of-love-or-why-we-swedes-are-so-lonely-234\">perception</a> that countries in Northern Europe are heavily individualistic and because of this people in these societies tend to be much lonelier. The data, however, does not support this claim.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>What is true is that in countries such as Denmark and Switzerland, it is <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE\">very common</a> for people to live alone. But contrary to what many believe, this does not translate into higher levels of self-reported loneliness.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Below we discuss the data in more detail, and show that this can be partly explained by the fact that loneliness and aloneness are just not the same. Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it\u2019s important not to conflate them.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Self-reported loneliness</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Loneliness describes a <em>subjective</em> feeling; this is conceptually distinct from <em>objective</em> physical isolation.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In the chart here we show estimates on self-reported feelings of loneliness among older adults. The data comes from various surveys asking people directly whether they often experience feelings of loneliness (e.g. <em>\u201cI have no-one with whom I can discuss important matters with\u201d</em>).</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The differences in the prevalence of loneliness across countries are very large. At the bottom of the list, Denmark, Switzerland, Sweden and the US all have rates below 30%, while at the top of the list Greece, Israel and Italy all have rates of close to or above 50%.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The data is available only for fifteen countries, but this sample does not suggest that <a href=\"https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Individualistic_culture\">‘rich individualistic societies’</a> are lonelier than others. In fact, at the very bottom are two countries in which the share of people living alone is among the <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE\">highest</a> in the world.<a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults\"></a></p>\n\n\n\n<iframe src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults\" style=\"width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;\"></iframe>\n\n\n\n<h4>Perceptions of social support</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Similar to loneliness, we can measure perceptions of social support by asking people directly. This is what the polling organization <em>Gallup</em> did in their flagship <a href=\"https://www.gallup.com/analytics/232838/world-poll.aspx\"><em>World Poll</em></a> survey. Specifically, they asked: <em>\u201cIf you were in trouble, do you have relatives or friends you can count on to help you whenever you need them, or not?\u201d</em></p>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart here presents the results from this survey, plotting the share of people who responded \u201cyes\u201d to this question.{ref} The data for this chart comes from a table published in the appendix of the OECD report \u201cHow’s Life? 2017 Measuring Well-Being\u201d, available online <a href=\"https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Differences across countries are not very large. The lowest and highest average levels of support corresponds to Mexico and Iceland, at 80% and 98% respectively. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>The second point that stands out is that, again, there\u2019s no support for the claim that richer countries that are considered to be more individualistic (e.g. North European countries) have lower levels of family and friendship support. </p>\n\n\n\n<iframe src=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/people-who-report-having-friends-or-relatives-they-can-count-on\" style=\"width: 100%; height: 600px; border: 0px none;\"></iframe>\n\n\n\n<h4>Distinguishing between aloneness and loneliness</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Are people who live alone more likely to say they feel lonely?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The researchers Caitlin Coyle and Elizabeth Dugan explored this question using data from the<em> Health and Retirement Study</em> in the US. In their <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275\">analysis</a>, covering almost 12,000 respondents over the years 2006-2008, they found that loneliness and social isolation are not highly correlated at the individual level.{ref}Coyle, C. E., & Dugan, E. (2012). Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. Journal of aging and health, 24(8), 1346-1363. Online <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0898264312460275\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>To be precise, Coyle and Dugan found a correlation coefficient of 0.2. That means the correlation is positive, and it is statistically larger than zero; but it\u2019s not very large in absolute terms. Their study does not provide the raw data, but in general, a scatter plot with correlation 0.2 is pretty much just a <a href=\"https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Correlation_and_dependence\">cloud of dots</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This means you cannot predict much of the variability in loneliness from observed differences in living arrangements. Some people who live alone are lonely, but many are not. Concluding that someone who lives alone must feel lonely is very often wrong.{ref}There\u2019s similar evidence from other countries. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the <em>Community Life Survey</em>, in which they ask about living arrangements and loneliness. This survey finds that while the reported frequency of loneliness is certainly higher among those who live alone, the correlation is not higher that of other \u2018risk factors\u2019 such as disability, age and marital status. You find more information in the ONS report <a href=\"https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Being alone and feeling lonely are not the same</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>People do not seem to be more lonely in societies that are <a href=\"https://www.hofstede-insights.com/country-comparison/finland,norway,sweden/\">traditionally labeled</a> as ‘individualistic’.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>What is true is that in these societies it is <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/one-person-households-vs-gdp-per-capita?country=DNK+CHE\">particularly common</a> for people to live alone. But being alone and feeling lonely don\u2019t always go hand in hand. Many people feel lonely even if they are not physically isolated; and many people who are physically isolated do not feel lonely. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>The aggregate statistics confirm this. Surveys that ask people about living arrangements, time use, and feelings of loneliness, find that solitude, by itself, does not predict feelings of loneliness. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>The fact that people in individualistic countries are not more likely to feel lonely may of course capture differences in expectations regarding social relations. But to the extent that loneliness is a subjective experience, these cross-country differences are still important to understand well-being.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Both loneliness and solitude deserve attention, but it\u2019s important not to conflate them.</p>\n\n\n\n<h3>Changes in loneliness over time: Is there a loneliness epidemic?</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>The media seems to have agreed that rich countries are experiencing a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019. There are literally <a href=\"https://www.google.com/search?biw=1304&bih=836&tbm=nws&sxsrf=ACYBGNTL_xMYGpHrk9r5bQaLi63lBs44IQ%3A1571695696286&ei=UCyuXcyEEbGd5wKmx4WwCg&q=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&oq=%22loneliness+epidemic%22&gs_l=psy-ab.3...4828.6314.0.6495.3.3.0.0.0.0.139.398.0j3.3.0....0...1c.1.64.psy-ab..0.0.0....0.6EEGBItbpoA\">thousands</a> of newspaper articles that use this exact expression.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>What is the evidence for this? The word \u2018epidemic\u2019 suggests that things are getting much worse and loneliness is increasing rapidly. But does the data in fact show that societies are becoming lonelier?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Despite the popularity of the claim, there is surprisingly no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>It is <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone\">true</a> that more people are living alone around the world. But loneliness and aloneness are not the same. As we explain in a <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/lonely-not-alone\">companion post</a>, spending time alone is not a good predictor of whether people feel lonely, or have weaker social support.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>As we explain later, today\u2019s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today\u2019s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than older adults in the past. Surveys covering older adults in other rich countries, including Finland, Germany, England and Sweden, point in the same direction \u2013 it\u2019s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations in these countries.{ref} See the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online <a href=\"https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Social connections \u2013 including contact with friends and family \u2013 are important for our <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/social-relations-health-and-well-being\">health and emotional welfare</a>, as well as for our <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/personal-relations-econ-outcomes\">material well-being</a>. Loneliness is indeed an important problem, but it\u2019s crucial to have a nuanced conversation based on facts. The headlines that claim we are witnessing a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019 are wrong and unhelpful.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Are young people more lonely than older adults?</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>One statistic that is often used to argue that loneliness is increasing, is that young people today are lonelier than older adults. This begs two questions: (i) Is it true that younger people are lonelier, and (ii) does this show that loneliness is increasing?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Let\u2019s start with the first question. In England, the Office for National Statistics conducts the <a href=\"https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/community-life-survey-2018-19\">Community Life Survey</a>, in which they ask people how often they feel lonely. In the bar chart here we show a breakdown of self-reported loneliness by age group.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>According to this data, those aged 16 to 24 are the group most likely to report feeling lonely, with 10% feeling lonely \u201coften or always\u201d. In contrast, those aged 65 years and older are the group least likely to report feeling lonely, with 3% feeling lonely \u201coften or always\u201d. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Many people tend to associate loneliness with older age, so this pattern might seem surprising. But surveys from several other rich countries have found the same. In <a href=\"http://archive.stats.govt.nz/browse_for_stats/people_and_communities/older_people/loneliness-in-nz-2010-NZGSS/loneliness-in-nz.aspx\">New Zealand</a>, <a href=\"http://files.kff.org/attachment/Report-Loneliness-and-Social-Isolation-in-the-United-States-the-United-Kingdom-and-Japan-An-International-Survey\">Japan</a> and the <a href=\"https://www.multivu.com/players/English/8294451-cigna-us-loneliness-survey/docs/IndexReport_1524069371598-173525450.pdf\">US</a>, young adults also report feeling lonely more often than older adults.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>So, yes, in rich countries we find that younger people are more likely to report feeling lonely.{ref}The emphasis on \u2018rich countries\u2019 is important. A study exploring self-reported loneliness across nine former Soviet countries, found that older adults report higher levels of loneliness. This shows it’s wrong to generalize from the available data, which is primarily from rich countries. For more details see: Stickley, A., Koyanagi, A., Roberts, B., Richardson, E., Abbott, P., Tumanov, S., & McKee, M. (2013). Loneliness: its correlates and association with health behaviours and outcomes in nine countries of the former Soviet Union. PloS one, 8(7), e67978. Online <a href=\"https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>What about the second question? Does this mean that loneliness is increasing? </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Here, the answer is \u2018no\u2019. Cross-sectional comparisons are not informative about changes <em>over time</em>, because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle. To be able to say something meaningful about changes in loneliness over time, we need to distinguish between changes for <em>individuals</em> over time (do people become lonelier as they get older?) and changes across generations (are people of the same age lonelier today than in the past?).</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Let\u2019s dig deeper and explore these questions separately. </p>\n\n\n\n<figure class=\"wp-block-image size-large\"><img loading=\"lazy\" width=\"731\" height=\"550\" src=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-731x550.png\" alt=\"\" class=\"wp-image-29000\" srcset=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-731x550.png 731w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-400x301.png 400w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-150x113.png 150w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-768x578.png 768w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-1536x1156.png 1536w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/Reported-loneliness-by-age-in-England-2048x1541.png 2048w\" sizes=\"(max-width: 731px) 100vw, 731px\" /></figure>\n\n\n\n<h4>Do we become lonelier as we get older?</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>To understand how loneliness changes across our life cycle, we need loneliness data from surveys that track the same individuals over time, up until old age. In a <a href=\"https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download\">study</a> published in the journal <em>Psychology and Aging</em>, Louise Hawkley and co-authors examine two such surveys, with data for adults older than 50 years in the US.{ref} Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online <a href=\"https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>They found that after age 50 \u2013 which is the earliest age of participants in their study \u2013 loneliness tended to decrease, until about 75, after which it began to increase again.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The authors explain in their paper that the increase in loneliness after 75 was explained by a decline in health and the loss of a spouse or partner. When adjusting for these factors, they found that loneliness continued declining into \u2018oldest old age\u2019.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This shows that there are two forces at play. On the one hand, there seems to be a direct relationship between age and loneliness, whereby loneliness <em>decreases</em> with age as our social expectations adapt, and we become <a href=\"https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1207/S15324834BASP2304_2?casa_token=885gubc-kPAAAAAA:gP8oVDQ-085Ftuh1-pvmG4Ms4-O58KDka4hjgK5Nq7RjS3ZsESymhhPo_owFbaf5MufXSx2LvFc0oA\">more selective</a> about relating with contacts who bring positive emotions. On the other hand, there seems to be an indirect association pushing in the opposite direction, whereby loneliness <em>increases</em> with age, because our health deteriorates and we lose relatives and friends. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>In our middle age the direct effect dominates, but once we enter advanced old age, the negative indirect effect starts dominating.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This complex relationship between age and loneliness shows why comparing old and young people at a given point in time is misleading. Cross-sectional comparisons are just not informative about the evolution of loneliness over time, because loneliness is not constant across the life cycle.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Loneliness trends over time: Are people lonelier today than in the past?</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>In the \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019 narrative, it is often implied that if we compare two individuals of the same age \u2013 one today and another one a generation ago \u2013 we would find that the one today is more likely to feel lonely. This is based on the idea that there have been societal changes \u2013 such as the <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone\">rise of living alone</a> \u2013 that make newer generations more likely to feel lonely.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In their <a href=\"https://psyarxiv.com/vx9nh/download\">study</a>, Louise Hawkley and co-authors searched for evidence of these \u2018cohort trends\u2019 in the US, but didn\u2019t find any. There was very little difference in self-reported loneliness of people born in different generations. Those that were born in 1920-1947 experienced the same changes of loneliness throughout their lives as those born in 1948-1965. It\u2019s not the case that loneliness is increasing across generations.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>An <a href=\"https://unherd.com/2019/05/is-the-epidemic-of-loneliness-fake-news/\">article</a> by Tom Chivers explains this result from the study very well: <em>\u201c[Hawkley and co-authors] found that \u201cnewer\u201d old people (baby boomers born 1948-1965) are no more likely to think of themselves as lonely than \u201colder\u201d old people (born 1920-1947), and that older people have not become more likely to think of themselves as lonely in the last decade (2005 \u2013 2016)… The average older person appears to be no more likely to be lonely than they were a decade ago.\u201d</em></p>\n\n\n\n<p>There are studies with data from other rich countries pointing in the same direction. In Sweden, repeated cross-sectional surveys with adults aged 85, 90, and 95 years old, found no increase in loneliness over a ten-year interval.{ref} Nyqvist, F., Cattan, M., Conradsson, M., N\u00e4sman, M., & Gustafsson, Y. (2017). Prevalence of loneliness over ten years among the oldest old. Scandinavian journal of public health, 45(4), 411-418. Available online <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1403494817697511\">here</a>. For similar results in other countries, including Finland, Germany and England, see the literature review in Hawkley, L. C., Wroblewski, K., Kaiser, T., Luhmann, M., & Schumm, L. P. (2019). Are U.S. older adults getting lonelier?: Age, period, and cohort differences. Psychology and Aging. Online <a href=\"https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0067978&type=printable\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>These studies find no evidence of cohort effects across older adults. What about evidence for adolescents?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In the aptly titled <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789\">paper</a> \u201cRethinking \u2018Generation Me\u2019: A Study of Cohort Effects from 1976-2006\u201d, the psychologists Kali Trzesniewski and Brent Donnellan used repeated cohort surveys to explore whether successive groups of high-school graduates were becoming more lonely in the US. They also found no evidence of cohort trends. Newer generations of high-school seniors were not more likely to report feelings of loneliness than earlier generations.{ref} Trzesniewski and Donnellan relied on data from the <a href=\"http://www.monitoringthefuture.org\">Monitoring the Future Project</a>, an ongoing survey of young Americans. Their analysis extends beyond loneliness. The paper abstract explains the results as follows: <em>\u201cSocial commentators have argued that changes over the last decades have coalesced to create a relatively unique generation of young people. However, using large samples of U.S. high-school seniors from 1976 to 2006 (Total N = 477,380), we found little evidence of meaningful change in egotism, self-enhancement, individualism, self-esteem, locus of control, hopelessness, happiness, life satisfaction, loneliness, antisocial behavior, time spent working or watching television, political activity, the importance of religion, and the importance of social status over the last 30 years.\u201d </em>The full reference of the paper is: Trzesniewski, K. H., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Rethinking \u201cGeneration Me\u201d: A study of cohort effects from 1976-2006. Perspectives on psychological science, 5(1), 58-75. Available online <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/1745691609356789\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The psychologists Matthew Clark, Natalie Loxton, and Stephanie Tobin <a href=\"https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1177/0146167214557007\">replicated</a> this analysis, using the same survey, but focusing on all age groups, not only high-school seniors. The chart here shows their results. They found no signs of increasing loneliness across all age groups. In fact, they found a very small but statistically significant <em>decline in loneliness </em>for high school students in the US.{ref} To measure loneliness, the project used a six-item questionnaire, where participants used a 1 (disagree) to 5 (agree) scale to respond to the following statements: \u2018\u2018<em>A lot of times I feel lonely</em>,\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018<em>There is always someone I can turn to if I need help (reverse scored)</em>,\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018<em>I often feel left out of things</em>,\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018<em>There is usually someone I can talk to if I need to (reverse scored)</em>,\u2019\u2019 \u2018\u2018<em>I often wish I had more good friends</em>,\u2019\u2019 and \u2018<em>\u2018I usually have a few good friends around that I can get together with (reverse scored).</em>\u2019\u2019 The project began collecting loneliness data for Grade 12 students in 1977, and data for Grades 8 and 10 in 1991. {/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p><em>(NB. The vertical axis in this chart is truncated, following the presentation in the original paper. The truncated axis is helpful to highlight the trend; but the takeaway is that the changes in levels are extremely small, so the trend is effectively flat in absolute terms, even if the slope is statistically different from zero).</em></p>\n\n\n\n<figure class=\"wp-block-image size-large\"><img loading=\"lazy\" width=\"786\" height=\"550\" src=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-786x550.png\" alt=\"\" class=\"wp-image-29001\" srcset=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-786x550.png 786w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-400x280.png 400w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-150x105.png 150w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-768x538.png 768w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-1536x1075.png 1536w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/12/declining-loneliness-of-US-students-2048x1434.png 2048w\" sizes=\"(max-width: 786px) 100vw, 786px\" /></figure>\n\n\n\n<h4>Loneliness deserves attention, but headlines that claim we are witnessing a \u2018loneliness epidemic\u2019 are not true, and are actually unhelpful</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The magazine <em>The Economist </em>wrote <a href=\"https://www.economist.com/international/2018/09/01/loneliness-is-a-serious-public-health-problem\">an article</a> in 2018 with the title \u201cLoneliness is a serious public-health problem\u201d. In one key paragraph, the article reads: \u201cHistorical data about loneliness are scant. But isolation does seem to be increasing, so loneliness may be too.\u201d </p>\n\n\n\n<p>The data we have found shows that this reasoning is actually incorrect. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Surveys from rich countries do not suggest there has been an increase in loneliness over time. Today\u2019s adolescents in the US do not seem to be more likely to report feeling lonely than adolescents from a couple of decades ago; and similarly, today\u2019s older adults in the US do not report higher loneliness than did adults of their age in the past.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>That\u2019s of course not to say we should not pay attention to these topics.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>It\u2019s important to provide support to people who <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults\">suffer from loneliness</a>, just as it is important to pay attention to the policy challenges that come from large societal changes such as the <a href=\"http://www.ourworldindata.org/living-alone\">rise of living alone</a>. However, inaccurate, over-simplified narratives are unhelpful to really understand these complex challenges.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>There is an epidemic of headlines that claim we are experiencing a \u201cloneliness epidemic\u201d, but there is no empirical support for the fact that loneliness is increasing, let alone spreading at epidemic rates.</p>\n\n\n\n<h2>Social media and social networks</h2>\n\n\n\n<h3>The rise of social media platforms</h3>\n\n\n\n\n\n<h3>Are Facebook and other social media platforms bad for our well-being?</h3>\n\n\n\n<p>Facebook, Youtube, Whatsapp, WeChat, and Instagram are the top five social media platforms globally, with over <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/users-by-social-media-platform\">one billion active users each</a>. In most rich countries the proportion of young people using online social networks <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/percentage-of-young-people-engaging-in-social-networking-online\">exceeds 90%</a> and teens spend on average <a href=\"https://ourworldindata.org/grapher/daily-time-spent-on-the-internet-by-young-people\">more than 4 hours online every day</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>We\u2019re repeatedly told in the news that social media is bad for us. The stories are often alarming, suggesting social media and smartphones are responsible for sweeping negative trends, <a href=\"https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression\">from rising suicide rates in the US</a>, to <a href=\"https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-families/social-media-mental-health-negative-effects-depression-anxiety-addiction-memory-a8307196.html\">widespread loss in memory, and reduced sleep and attention spans</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>These worrying headlines often go together with implicit or explicit recommendations to limit the amount of time we spend on social media. Indeed, smartphones today come with built-in \u201cscreen time\u201d apps that let us track and limit how much time we spend online.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>At the same time, most of us would agree that digital social media platforms can make our lives easier in many ways \u2013 opening doors to new information, connecting us with people who are far away, and helping us to be more flexible with work.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>What does the research tell us about the causal impact of social media use on our well-being?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In a nutshell: From my reading of the scientific literature, I do not believe that the available evidence today supports the sweeping newspaper headlines.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Yes, there is evidence suggesting a causal negative effect, but the size of these causal effects is heterogeneous and much, much smaller than the news headlines suggest.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>There are still plenty of good reasons to reflect on the impact of social media in society, and there is much we can all learn to make better use of these complex digital platforms. But this requires going beyond universal claims.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Let\u2019s take a look at the evidence.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Comparisons across individuals</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Most of the news stories that claim social media has a negative impact on well-being rely on data from surveys comparing individuals with different levels of social media use as evidence. In the chart, I show one concrete example of this type of correlational analysis.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart plots the average amount of time that people spend on social media each day, among people who are and aren\u2019t happy with the amount of time spent on these platforms.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The data comes from an app called <a href=\"https://inthemoment.io/\">Moment</a>, which tracks the amount of time users spend on social media platforms on their smartphones. The app also asks people a yes/no question: \u201cAre you happy with your time spent?\u201d</p>\n\n\n\n<p>As we can see there is quite a bit of heterogeneity across platforms, but the pattern is clear: People who say they are happy with how much time they spend on social media spend less time on these platforms. Or put differently, using social media more heavily is correlated with less satisfaction.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This is certainly interesting, but we should be careful not to jump to conclusions \u2013 the correlation actually raises as many questions as it answers.{ref}A concrete example of how this correlational evidence is reported in the news can be found in <a href=\"https://www.economist.com/graphic-detail/2018/05/18/how-heavy-use-of-social-media-is-linked-to-mental-illness\">this article from The Economist</a>, where a very similar chart is presented under the headline \u201cHow heavy use of social media is linked to mental illness\u201d.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<figure class=\"wp-block-image\"><img loading=\"lazy\" width=\"608\" height=\"550\" src=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/09/TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-\u2013-happy-vs-unhappy-users-608x550.png\" alt=\"\" class=\"wp-image-24578\" srcset=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/09/TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-\u2013-happy-vs-unhappy-users-608x550.png 608w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/09/TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-\u2013-happy-vs-unhappy-users-150x136.png 150w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/09/TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-\u2013-happy-vs-unhappy-users-400x362.png 400w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2019/09/TIme-spent-on-social-media-apps-\u2013-happy-vs-unhappy-users-768x695.png 768w\" sizes=\"(max-width: 608px) 100vw, 608px\" /></figure>\n\n\n\n<p>Does this pattern hold if we control for user characteristics like age and gender? Would we get similar results if we focused on other measures of well-being beyond \u2018happy with time spent\u2019?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The answer to both questions is \u2018no\u2019. Depending on what outcome variables you focus on, and depending on which demographic characteristics you account for, you will get a different result. It is therefore not surprising that some empirical academic studies have found negative correlations; while others actually report positive correlations.{ref}Here are some examples of studies reporting a positive correlation between social media use and subjective well-being: – Kim, J. and Lee, J. (2011). The facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number of face- book friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. CyberPsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(6):359\u2013364 – Gonzales, A. L. and Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my facebook wall: Effects of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(1-2):79\u201383. – Valenzuela, S., Park, N., and Kee, K. F. (2009). Is there social capital in a social network site?: Facebook use and college students\u2019 life satisfaction, trust, and participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14(4):875\u2013901.{/ref} </p>\n\n\n\n<p>Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski <a href=\"https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-018-0506-1\">published a paper</a> earlier this year in the journal <em>Nature</em> where they illustrated that given the flexibility to analyze the data (i.e. given the number of possible choices researchers have when it comes to processing and interpreting the vast data from these large surveys), scientists <a href=\"https://socialsciences.nature.com/users/200472-amy-orben/posts/42763-beyond-cherry-picking\">could have written thousands of papers</a> describing positive, negative and non-significant associations. Different ways of measuring well-being and social media use will yield different results, even for the same population.{ref} It\u2019s also important to mention that the correlations between social media use and wellbeing are not generally comparable across countries and time. For example, commentators<a href=\"https://www.theguardian.com/society/2018/may/24/smartphone-teen-suicide-mental-health-depression\"> often highlight</a> that depression and suicide rates among adolescents in the US have been going up ever since the rise of smartphone, and that this is a clear red flag. Yet as Andrew Przybylski explained <a href=\"http://rationallyspeakingpodcast.org/show/rs-237-andy-przybylski-on-is-screen-time-bad-for-you.html\">in a recent interview</a>, this is just not a general observation: <em>\u201cYou don’t see things like it in more tech-saturated countries, or in other industrialized countries. You don’t see, two or three years ahead of the United States, the South Koreans and the Japanese having spiking rates of self-harm or depression\u201d</em>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Even the answers to some of the most fundamental questions are unclear: Do we actually know in which direction the relationship might be going? Does frequent social media use translate into lower happiness, or is it the other way around \u2013 are anxious, stressed or depressed people particularly prone to use social media?</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This takes us to another branch of the literature: longitudinal studies that track individuals over time to measure changes in social media use and well-being.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Studies of social media use and well-being over time</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>One longitudinal study that has received much attention on this subject was <a href=\"https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143\">published</a> by Holly Shakya and Nicholas Christakis in the <em>American Journal of Epidemiology</em> in 2017. It used data from a survey that tracked a group of 5,208 Americans over the period 2013 – 2015, and found an increase in Facebook activity was associated with a future <em>decrease</em> in reported mental health.{ref}The full reference is: Shakya, H. B., & Christakis, N. A. (2017). Association of Facebook use with compromised well-being: A longitudinal study. <em>American journal of epidemiology</em>, <em>185</em>(3), 203-211. Online <a href=\"https://academic.oup.com/aje/article/185/3/203/2915143\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Two years later, Amy Orben, Tobias Dienlin and Andrew Przybylski <a href=\"https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf\">published a paper</a> in the <em>Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences</em> using a similar source of data. They relied on a longitudinal survey from the UK covering 12,672 teenagers over the period 2009 – 2016, and reached a different conclusion. They found that there was a small and reciprocal relationship: social media use predicted small decreases in life satisfaction; but it was also the case that decreasing life satisfaction predicted subsequent increases in social media use.{ref}The full reference is: Orben, A., Dienlin, T., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Social media\u2019s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(21), 10226-10228. Online <a href=\"https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/116/21/10226.full.pdf\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Summarizing their research in <em>The Guardian</em>, Amy Orben and Andrew Przybylski <a href=\"https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2019/jul/07/too-much-screen-time-hurts-kids-where-is-evidence\">explained</a>: <em>\u201cwe did find some small trends over time \u2013 these were mostly clustered in data provided by teenage girls\u2026 But \u2013 and this is key \u2013 it\u2019s not an exaggeration to say that these effects were minuscule by the standards of science and trivial if you want to inform personal parenting decisions. Our results indicated that 99.6% of the variability in adolescent girls\u2019 satisfaction with life had nothing to do with how much they used social media.\u201d</em></p>\n\n\n\n<p>In their paper Orben and co-authors argue again that these large datasets allow many different types of empirical tests; so it is <a href=\"https://slatestarcodex.com/2014/12/12/beware-the-man-of-one-study/\">natural to expect conflicting results across studies</a>, particularly if there is noise in measurement and the true effect sizes are small.{ref}What\u2019s more, if the cutoff for a statistically significant finding is 5% and you test 100 independent hypotheses of which in reality none are true, you will (in expectation) still find 5 significant associations in either direction simply by chance. Here is a good example and discussion of how this works: <a href=\"https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1\">https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/science-isnt-broken/#part1</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p> Orben and co-authors tested thousands of empirical tests and indeed, some of these tests could have been interpreted on their own as evidence of a strong negative effect for social media \u2013 but clearly the broader picture is important. When looking at the results from all their thousands of tests, they concluded that social media effects were nuanced, small at best and reciprocal over time.{ref}One more piece of evidence that confirms this conclusion, comes from a more recent long-run study that tracked 500 adolescents for eight consecutive years, between the ages of 13 and 20, and found that <em> “increased time spent on social media was not associated with increased mental health issues across development.”</em> The full reference is Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L., & Booth, M. (2019). Does time spent using social media impact mental health?: An eight year longitudinal study. <em>Computers in Human Behavior</em>, 106160. Available online <a href=\"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0747563219303723\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>Facebook experiments</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>Establishing causal impacts through observational studies that track the well-being of individuals over time is difficult. </p>\n\n\n\n<p>First, there are measurement issues. Long-run surveys that track people are expensive and impose a high burden on participants, so they do not allow in-depth high-frequency data collection, and instead focus on broad trends across a wide range of topics. Orben and co-authors, for example, rely on the <a href=\"https://www.understandingsociety.ac.uk/\"><em>Understanding Society Survey</em></a> from the UK, which covers a wide range of themes such as family life, education, employment, finance, health and wellbeing. Specifically on social media use, this survey only ask how many hours teenagers remember using apps during normal weekdays, which is of course an informative but noisy measure of actual use (a fact that Orben and co-authors mention in their paper).</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Second, there are limitations from unobservable variables. Frequent users of social media are likely different from less frequent users in ways that are hard to measure \u2013 no matter how many questions you include in a survey, there will always be relevant factors you cannot account for in the analysis.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Given these limitations, an obvious alternative is to run an experiment: you can, for example, offer people money to stop using Facebook for a while and then check the effect by comparing these \u201ctreated participants\u201d against a control group that is allowed to continue using Facebook as usual.{ref} Experiments like this will tell us the short-run effect among experiment participants. This is of course not a silver bullet \u2013 one could raise concerns about how generalizable the estimates are (e.g. Are participants representative of the general population? Do effects remain over time?). No method is perfect, but experiments do address the issue of causality, so they provide critical evidence to understand the link between social media and well-being.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Several recent papers followed this approach. Here I\u2019ll discuss <a href=\"https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf\">one of them in particular</a>, because I find its approach particularly compelling. The analysis relies on a much larger sample than other experiments, and the researchers registered a <a href=\"https://www.socialscienceregistry.org/trials/3409\">pre-analysis plan</a> to insure themselves against the \u2018analytical flexibility\u2019 criticisms discussed above.{ref} The full reference is: Allcott, H., Braghieri, L., Eichmeyer, S., & Gentzkow, M. (2019). The welfare effects of social media (No. w25514). National Bureau of Economic Research. Available online <a href=\"https://www.nber.org/papers/w25514.pdf\">here</a>.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>This experiment was done by four economists: Hunt Allcott, Sarah Eichmeyer, Luca Braghieri and Matthew Gentzkow. They recruited 2753 Facebook users in the US, and randomly selected half of them to stop using Facebook for four weeks. They found that deactivating Facebook led to small but statistically significant improvements in some measures of self-reported well-being.{ref} They recruit participants by giving them electronic gift cards. Loosely speaking, the idea was that participants were paid to play a lottery: Everybody received the monetary incentive, but only those who \u2018lost the lottery\u2019 had to deactivate their account.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The chart shows a summary of their estimated effect sizes. As we can see, for all measures the effects are small (amounting to only around a tenth of the standard deviation of the studied variable), and in most cases the effects are actually not statistically significant (the \u2018whiskers\u2019 denoting 95% confidence intervals often include an effect of size zero).{ref} The paper provides the following definition for the variables: <em>The happiness variable is the average response to two questions from the Subjective Happiness Scale (Lyubomirsky and Lepper 1999), asking how happy participants were over the past four weeks and how happy they were compared to their peers. Life satisfaction is the sum of responses to three questions from the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al. 1985), such as the level of agreement with the statement, \u201cDuring the past 4 weeks, I was satisfied with my life.\u201d Loneliness is the Three-Item Loneliness Scale (Hughes et al. 2004). Finally, depressed, anxious, absorbed, and bored reflect how much of the time during the past four weeks respondents felt each emotion, using questions from the European Social Survey well-being module (Huppert et al. 2009). The daily text messages allowed us to measure the aspects of subjective well-being that are most important to record in the moment instead of retrospectively. This approach builds on the Experience Sampling Method of Csikszentmihalyi and Larson (2014) and Stone and Shiffman (1994). The variable SMS happiness is the answer to the question, \u201cOverall, how happy do you feel right now on a scale from 1 (not at all happy) to 10 (completely happy)?\u201d The variable SMS positive emotion is an indicator variable for whether the participant reports a positive emotion when asked, \u201cWhat best describes how you felt over the last ten minutes?\u201d, with possible responses such as \u201cangry,\u201d \u201cworried,\u201d \u201cloving/tender,\u201d etc. Finally, SMS not lonely uses the answer to the question, \u201cHow lonely are you feeling right now on a scale from 1 (not at all lonely) to 10 (very lonely)?\u201d</em>{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Allcott and co-authors also compare the treatment effects against the observational correlations in their sample and conclude: \u201c<em>the magnitudes of our causal effects are far smaller than those we would have estimated using the correlational approach of much prior literature\u201d</em>.{ref} To get a sense of the magnitudes of these effects, the authors discuss effect sizes in their original units, focusing on the measures with the largest effects. Happiness is the average response to two questions on a scale from 1 (not a very happy person) to 7 (a very happy person). The control group endline average is 4.47 out of a possible 7, and deactivation caused an average increase of 0.12. This is of course very, very small. In fact, the authors explain that given how small these effect sizes are, they would have been unlikely to have sufficient statistical power to detect any effects if they had used sample sizes similar to those from previous experiments in this field, all of which are much smaller.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The relatively small experimental effect of social media use on subjective well-being has been replicated. <a href=\"https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462\">Another experiment</a> conducted almost at the same time and with a very similar approach, produced similar results.{ref} In <a href=\"https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462\">this other experiment</a>, quitting Facebook for a week led to a small but statistically significant decrease in self-reported feelings of depression, yet there was no significant effect on other measures of life satisfaction. The full reference is: Mosquera, R., Odunowo, M. M., McNamara, T., Guo, X., & Petrie, R. (2018). The Economic Effects of Facebook. {/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<figure class=\"wp-block-image size-large\"><img loading=\"lazy\" width=\"530\" height=\"550\" src=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2022/12/subjective-well-being-1-v2-full-530x550.png\" alt=\"\" class=\"wp-image-55044\" srcset=\"https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2022/12/subjective-well-being-1-v2-full-530x550.png 530w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2022/12/subjective-well-being-1-v2-full-385x400.png 385w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2022/12/subjective-well-being-1-v2-full-145x150.png 145w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2022/12/subjective-well-being-1-v2-full-768x797.png 768w, https://owid.cloud/app/uploads/2022/12/subjective-well-being-1-v2-full.png 1240w\" sizes=\"(max-width: 530px) 100vw, 530px\" /></figure>\n\n\n\n<h4>Mechanisms</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>In the US, where many of these studies have been conducted, roughly two-thirds of people <a href=\"https://www.journalism.org/2018/09/10/news-use-across-social-media-platforms-2018/\">get news from social media</a>, and these platforms have already become a <a href=\"https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/12/10/social-media-outpaces-print-newspapers-in-the-u-s-as-a-news-source/\">more widely accessed source of news than print newspapers</a>.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>I think this link between social media, news consumption and well-being is key.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>In their experiment, Allcott and coauthors found that quitting Facebook did not lead people to use alternative online or offline news sources; so those in the treatment group reported spending less time consuming news overall. This tells us that the effect of social media on well-being is not only relatively small, but also likely mediated by the specific types of content and information that people are exposed to.{ref}This effect has also been replicated: The other contemporary experiment that followed a similar approach (<a href=\"https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3312462\">Mosquera et al. 2019</a>), found that individuals facing a Facebook restriction reduced their news consumption.{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The fact that news consumption via social media might be an important factor affecting well-being is not surprising if we consider that news are typically biased towards negative content, and there is <a href=\"https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract\">empirical research</a> suggesting people are triggered, at a physiological level, when exposed to negative news content.{ref} A new paper published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences concludes that all around the world, the average human is more physiologically activated by negative than by positive news stories. The authors come to this conclusion through an experiment: Participants watched 7 randomly ordered BBC World News stories on a laptop computer while wearing noise-cancelling headphones and sensors on their fingers to capture skin conductance and blood volume pulse. The experiment was conducted with 1,156 participants across 17 countries: Brazil, Canada, Chile, China, Denmark, France, Ghana, India, Israel, Italy, Japan, New Zealand, Russia, Senegal, Sweden, the United Kingdom, and the United States. The full reference is Soroka, S., Fournier P., & Nir L., (2019) Cross-national evidence of a negativity bias in psychophysiological reactions to news. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1908369116. Available online <a href=\"https://www.pnas.org/content/early/2019/08/27/1908369116.abstract\">here</a>.<br>{/ref}</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Building and reinforcing a scary overarching narrative around \u201cthe terrible negative effects of social media on well-being\u201d is unhelpful because this fails to recognise that social media is a large and evolving ecosystem where billions of people interact and consume information in many different ways.</p>\n\n\n\n<h4>What are the key takeaways?</h4>\n\n\n\n<p>The first takeaway is that the association between social media and well-being is complex and reciprocal, which means that simple correlations can be misleading. A careful analysis of survey data reveals that, yes, there is a correlation between social media and well-being; but the relationship works both ways. This becomes clear from the longitudinal studies: Higher use of social media predicts decreases in life satisfaction; and decreasing life satisfaction also predicts subsequent increases in social media use.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>The second takeaway is that the causal effect of social media on well-being is likely small for the average person. The best empirical evidence suggests the impact is much smaller than many news stories suggest and most people believe.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>There is much to be learned about how to make better use of these digital platforms, and there is an important discussion to be had about the opportunity costs of spending a large fraction of our time online. But for this we need to look beyond the sweeping newspaper headlines.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>We need research with more granular data to unpack diverse use patterns, to understand the different effects that certain types of content have on specific population groups. Time alone is a poor metric to gauge effects. As Andrew Przybylski put it: nobody would argue we should study the causes of obesity by investigating \u2018food time\u2019.</p>\n\n\n\n<p>Going forward, the conversation in policy and the news should be much more about strategies to promote positive content and interactions, than about one-size-fits-all restrictions on social media \u2018screen time\u2019.<br></p>\n\n\n\n<h2>Data Sources</h2>\n\n\n\n<h4>Snell, K. D. M. (2017). The rise of living alone and loneliness in history. Social History, 42(1), 2-28.</h4>\n\n\n\n<ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Estimates of the share of single-person households across different places and times, using a selection of the available historical records and more recent census data</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Selected towns and cities</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href=\"https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093\">https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/03071022.2017.1256093</a></li></ul>\n\n\n\n<h4> Cigna – Loneliness Report</h4>\n\n\n\n<ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Survey of 20,000 Americans</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Self-reported loneliness</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> US</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href=\"https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america\">https://www.cigna.com/about-us/newsroom/studies-and-reports/loneliness-epidemic-america</a> </li></ul>\n\n\n\n<h4>Office for National Statistics – Loneliness Report</h4>\n\n\n\n<ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Community Life Survey</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Self-reported loneliness</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> UK</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href=\"https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/lonelinesswhatcharacteristicsandcircumstancesareassociatedwithfeelinglonely/2018-04-10\">https://www.ons.gov.uk</a></li></ul>\n\n\n\n<h4>World Value Survey</h4>\n\n\n\n<ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Representative national surveys</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Share of people who interact with friends at least once per month</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Selected countries</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href=\"http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/\">http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/</a></li></ul>\n\n\n\n<h4>United Nations Database on Household Size and Composition 2018</h4>\n\n\n\n<ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Percentage of one-person households as a ratio of total households</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Global, by country</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href=\"https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840\">https://population.un.org/Household/index.html#/countries/840</a></li></ul>\n\n\n\n<h4>OECD \u2013 How’s Life? 2017 Measuring Well-being</h4>\n\n\n\n<ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Percentage of people who report that they have friends or relatives whom they can count on in times of trouble</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Global, by country</li><li><strong>Link</strong>: <a href=\"https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en\">https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/economics/how-s-life-2017_how_life-2017-en</a></li></ul>\n\n\n\n<h4>OECD \u2013 Society at a Glance</h4>\n\n\n\n<ul><li><strong>Data Source:</strong> Multiple sources</li><li><strong> Description of available measures:</strong> Social media use</li><li><strong>Geographical coverage:</strong> Global, by country</li><li><strong>Link:</strong> <a href=\"https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en\">https://doi.org/10.1787/soc_glance-2019-en</a></li></ul>\n\n\n\n<p></p>\n", "protected": false }, "excerpt": { "rendered": "Family and friends are important for our well-being. In this article we explore data on loneliness and social connections, and review available evidence on the link between social connections and well-being.", "protected": false }, "date_gmt": "2020-02-14T05:42:00", "modified": "2023-01-16T21:07:07", "template": "", "categories": [ 52, 237 ], "menu_order": 146, "ping_status": "closed", "authors_name": [ "Esteban Ortiz-Ospina" ], "modified_gmt": "2023-01-16T21:07:07", "comment_status": "closed", "featured_media": 23957, "featured_media_paths": { "thumbnail": "/app/uploads/2019/07/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults-1-150x106.png", "medium_large": "/app/uploads/2019/07/self-reported-loneliness-older-adults-1-768x542.png" } } |